Federal Register: April 18, 2001 (Volume 66, Number 75)
Additional Scientific Data Considered by the Drug Enforcement
Administration in Evaluating Jon Gettman’s Petition To Initiate
Rulemaking Proceedings To Reschedule Marijuana
Drug and Chemical Evaluation Section, Office of Diversion Control, Drug
Enforcement Administration, March 2001
Introduction
On July 10, 1995, Jon Gettman petitioned the Drug Enforcement
Administration (DEA) to initiate rulemaking proceedings to reschedule
marijuana. Marijuana is currently listed in schedule I of the
Controlled Substances Act (CSA).
Mr. Gettman proposed that DEA promulgate a rule stating that
“there is no scientific evidence that [marijuana has] sufficient abuse
potential to warrant schedule I or II status under the [CSA].”
In accordance with the CSA, DEA gathered the necessary data and, on
December 17, 1997, forwarded that information along with Mr. Gettman’s
petition to the Department of Health and Human Services (HHS) for a
scientific and medical evaluation and scheduling recommendation. On
January 17, 2001, HHS forwarded to DEA its scientific and medical
evaluation and scheduling recommendation. The CSA requires DEA to
determine whether the HHS scientific and medical evaluation and
scheduling recommendation and “all other relevant data” constitute
substantial evidence that the drug should be rescheduled as proposed in
the petition. 21 U.S.C. 811(b). This document contains an explanation
of the “other relevant data” that DEA considered.
In deciding whether to grant a petition to initiate rulemaking
proceedings, DEA must consider eight factors specified in 21 U.S.C.
811(c). The information contained in this document is organized
according to these eight factors.
(1) Its Actual or Relative Potential for Abuse
Evaluation of the abuse potential of a drug is obtained, in part,
from studies in the scientific and medical literature. There are many
preclinical indicators of a drug’s behavioral and psychological effects
that, when taken together, provide an accurate prediction of the human
abuse liability. Specifically, these include assessments of the
discriminative stimulus effects, reinforcing effects, conditioned
stimulus effect, effects on operant response rates, locomotor activity,
effects on food intake and other behaviors, and the development of
tolerance and dependence (cf., Brady et al., 1990; Preston et al.,
1997). Clinical studies of the subjective and reinforcing effects in
substance abusers, interviews with substance abusers, clinical
interviews with medical professionals, and epidemiological studies
provide quantitative data on abuse liability in humans and some
indication of actual abuse trends (cf., deWit and Griffiths, 1991).
Evidence of actual abuse and patterns of abuse are obtained from a
number of substance abuse databases, and reports of diversion and
trafficking. Specifically, data from Drug Abuse Warning Network (DAWN),
Poison
[[Page 20054]]
Control Centers, System To Retrieve Investigational Drug Evidence
(STRIDE), seizures and declarations from U.S. Customs, DEA Drug Theft
Reports and other diversion and trafficking data bases are indicators
of the pattern, scope, duration and significance of abuse.
Reinforcing Effects in Animals
As described by the petitioner, the preponderance of preclinical
studies using animal models had, to recently, shown that \9\-
THC had minimal activity in behavioral paradigms predictive of
reinforcing efficacy (i.e., self-administration paradigms; Harris et
al., 1974; Pickens et al., 1973; Deneau and Kaymakcalan, 1971). In
general, \9\-THC had been shown to be relatively ineffective
in maintaining self-administration behavior by either the intravenous
or oral routes (Kaymakcalan, 1973; Harris et al., 1974; Carney et al.,
1977; Mansbach et al., 1994). Under limited experimental parameters,
\9\-THC self-administration was demonstrated after animals
were either first trained to self-administer PCP, after a chronic
cannabinoid history was established or when maintained at 80% reduced
body weight (Pickens et al., 1973; Deneau and Kaymakcalan, 1971;
Takahashi and Singer, 1979). However, Tanda, Munzar and Goldberg of the
Intramural Preclinical Pharmacology Section of the NIDA (2000) have
clearly demonstrated that THC can act as a strong reinforcer of drug-
taking behavior in an experimental animal model, the squirrel monkey,
as it does in humans. The self-administration behavior was comparable
in intensity to that maintained by cocaine under identical conditions
and was obtained using a range of doses similar to those self-
administered by humans smoking a single marijuana cigarette.
Although the neuropharmacological actions of \9\-THC
suggest a powerful brain substrate underlying its rewarding and
euphorigenic effects, behavioral studies of \9\-THC’s
rewarding effects had been inconclusive. Several reasons for the
previous inability by a number of laboratories to demonstrate self-
administration of \9\-THC in animals may be its relatively
slow-onset, its long-lasting behavioral effects and its insolubility in
physiological saline or water for injection (Mansbach et al., 1994).
Similar findings have been found in the animal literature with
nicotine–an avid reinforcer in humans. The strength of THC, like
nicotine, as a reinforcer in animals may be more dependent on
supplementary strengthening by ancillary stimuli than is the case for
other drugs (cf. Henningfield, 1984).
In other behavioral and pharmacological tests used to assess
reinforcing efficacy, \9\-THC produced significant effects.
Specifically, \9\-THC augments responding for intracranial
self-stimulation by decreasing the reinforcing threshold for brain
stimulation reward. It also dose-dependently enhances dopamine efflux
in forebrain nuclei associated with reward and this enhanced efflux
occurs locally in the terminal fields within brain reward pathways
(Gardner and Lowinson, 1991; Gardner, 1992; Chen et al., 1993, 1994).
In conditioned place preference procedures, \9\-THC (2.0 and
4.0 mg/kg, i.p.) produced significant dose-dependent increases in
preference for the drug paired chamber, the magnitude of which was
similar to that seen with 5.0 mg/kg cocaine and 4.0 mg/kg morphine
(Leprore et al., 1995). However, \9\-THC also produced a
conditioned place aversion and conditioned taste aversion (Leprore et
al., 1995; Parker and Gillies, 1995). The development of taste
aversions with drug administrations that also produce place preferences
have been described as somewhat of a “drug paradox” by Goudie;
however, this has been found to occur within the “therapeutic window”
of all known drugs of abuse (cf Goudie, 1987). Goudie has concluded
that drugs can possess both reinforcing and aversive properties at the
same doses. This fact may underlie the reciprocal relationship between
the behavioral effects of THC, CBD, and THC+CBD combinations, discussed
below.
Drug Discrimination in Animals
Preclinical drug discrimination studies with \9\-THC are
predictive of the subjective effects of cannabinoid drugs in humans and
serve as animal models of marijuana and THC intoxication in humans
(Balster and Prescott, 1992; Wiley et al., 1993b, 1995). In a variety
of species it has been found that \9\-THC shares
discriminative stimulus effects with cannabinoids that bind to CNS
cannabinoid receptors with high affinity (Compton et al., 1993; Jarbe
et al., 1989; Gold et al., 1992; Wiley et al., 1993b, 1995b; Jarbe and
Mathis, 1992) and that are psychoactive in humans (Balster and
Prescott, 1992). Furthermore, recent studies show that the
discriminative stimulus effects of \9\-THC are mediated via
the CB1 receptor subtype (Perio et al., 1996).
Chronic \9\-THC administration to rats produced tolerance
to the discriminative stimulus effects of \9\-THC, but not to
its response rate disruptions. Specifically, tolerance to the stimulus
effects of \9\-THC increased 40-fold when supplemental doses
of up to 120 mg/kg/day \9\-THC were administered under
conditions of suspended training (Wiley et al., 1993a).
The discriminative stimulus effects of \9\-THC appear to
be pharmacologically specific as non-cannabinoid drugs typically do not
elicit cannabimimetic effects in drug discrimination studies (Browne
and Weissman, 1981; Balster and Prescott, 1992, Gold et al., 1992;
Barrett et al., 1995; Wiley et al., 1995a). Furthermore, these studies
show that high doses of \9\-THC produce marked response rate
disruption, immobility, ataxia, sedation and ptosis in rhesus monkeys
and rats (Wiley et al., 1993b; Gold et al., 1992; Martin et al., 1995).
Clinical Abuse Potential
Both marijuana and THC can serve as positive reinforcers in humans.
Marijuana and \9\-THC produced profiles of behavioral and
subjective effects that were similar regardless of whether the
marijuana was smoked or taken orally, as marijuana in brownies, or
orally as THC-containing capsules, although the time course of effects
differed substantially. There is a large clinical literature
documenting the subjective, reinforcing, discriminative stimulus, and
physiological effects of marijuana and THC and relating these effects
to the abuse potential of marijuana and THC (e.g., Chait et al., 1988;
Lukas et al., 1995; Kamien et al., 1994; Chait and Burke, 1994; Chait
and Pierri, 1992; Foltin et al., 1990; Azorlosa et al., 1992; Kelly et
al., 1993, 1994; Chait and Zacny, 1992; Cone et al., 1988; Mendelson
and Mello, 1984).
These listed studies represent a fraction of the studies performed
to evaluate the abuse potential of marijuana and THC. In general, these
studies demonstrate that marijuana and THC dose-dependently increases
heart rate and ratings of “high” and “drug liking”, and alters
behavioral performance measures (e.g., Azorlosa et al., 1992; Kelly et
al., 1993, 1994; Chait and Zacny, 1992; Kamien et al., 1994; Chait and
Burke, 1994; Chait and Pierri, 1992; Foltin et al., 1990; Cone et al.,
1988; Mendelson and Mello, 1984). Marijuana also serves as a
discriminative stimulus in humans and produces euphoria and alterations
in mood. These subjective changes were used by the subjects as the
basis for the discrimination from placebo (Chait et al., 1988).
In addition, smoked marijuana administration resulted in multiple
brief episodes of euphoria that were paralleled by rapid transient
increases in EEG alpha power (Lukas et al., 1995);
[[Page 20055]]
these EEG changes are thought to be related to CNS processes of
reinforcement (Mello, 1983).
To help elucidate the relationship between the rise and fall of
plasma THC and the self-reported psychotropic effects, Harder &
Rietbrock (1997) measured both the plasma levels of THC and the
psychological “high” obtained from smoking a marijuana cigarette
containing 1% THC. As can be seen from these data, a rise in plasma THC
concentrations results in a corresponding increase in the subjectively
reported feelings of being “high”. However, as THC levels drop the
subjectively reported feelings of “high” remain elevated. The
subjective effects seem to lag behind plasma THC levels. Similarly,
Harder and Rietbrock compared lower doses of 0.3% THC-containing and
0.1% THC-containing cigarettes in human subjects.
As can be clearly seen by these data, even low doses of marijuana,
containing 1%, 0.3% and even 0.1% THC, typically referred to as “non-
active”, are capable of producing subjective reports and physiological
markers of being “high’.
THC and its major metabolite, 11-OH-THC, have similar psychoactive
and pharmacokinetic profiles in man ( Wall et al., 1976; DiMarzo et
al., 1998; Lemberger et al., 1972). Perez-Reyes et al. (1972) reported
that THC and 11-OH-THC were equipotent in generating a “high” in
human volunteers. However, the metabolite, 11-OH-THC, crosses the
blood-brain barrier faster than the parent THC compound (Ho et al.,
1973; Perez-Reyes et al., 1976). Therefore, the changes in THC plasma
concentrations in humans may not be the best predictive marker for the
subjective and physiological effects of marijuana in humans. Cocchetto
et al. (1981) have used hysteresis plots to clearly demonstrate that
plasma THC concentration is a poor predictor of simultaneous occurring
physiological (heart rate) and psychological (“high”) pharmacological
effects. Cocchetto et al. demonstrated that the time course of
tachycardia and psychological responses lagged behind the plasma THC
concentration-time profile. As recently summarized by Martin & Hall
(1997, 1998)
There is no linear relationship between blood [THC] levels and
pharmacological effects with respect to time, a situation that
hampers the prediction of cannabis-induced impairment based on THC
blood levels (p90).
Physical Dependence in Animals
There are reports that abrupt withdrawal from
9-THC can produce a mild spontaneous withdrawal
syndrome in animals, including increased motor activity and grooming in
rats, decreased seizure threshold in mice, increased aggressiveness,
irritability and altered operant performance in rhesus monkeys (cf.,
Pertwee, 1991). The failure to observe profound withdrawal signs
following abrupt discontinuation of the drug may be due to
9-THC’s long half-life in plasma and slowly waning
levels of drug that continue to permit receptor adaptation.
Recently the discovery of a cannabinoid receptor antagonist
demonstrates that a profound precipitated withdrawal syndrome can be
produced in 9-THC tolerant animals after twice
daily injections (Tsou et al., 1995) or continuous infusion (Aceto et
al., 1995, 1996).
Physical Dependence in Humans
Signs of withdrawal in humans have been demonstrated after studies
with marijuana and 9-THC. Although the intensity of
the withdrawal syndrome is related to the daily dose and frequency of
administration, in general, the signs of 9-THC
withdrawal have been relatively mild (cf., Pertwee, 1991). This
withdrawal syndrome has been compared to that of short-term, low dose
treatment with opioids, sedatives, or ethanol, and includes changes in
mood, sleep, heart rate, body temperature, and appetite. Other signs
such as irritability, restlessness, tremor, mild nausea, hot flashes
and sweating have also been noted (cf., Jones, 1980, 1983).
Chait, Fischman, & Schuster (1985) have demonstrated an acute
withdrawal syndrome or “hangover” occurring approximately 9 hours
after a single marijuana smoking episode. Significant changes occurred
on two subjective measures and on a time production task. In 1973,
Cousens & DiMascio reported a similar “hangover” effect from acute
administrations of 9-THC. The hangover phenomenon
or continued “high”, in the Cousens & DiMascio study, occurred 9 hrs
after drug administration and was associated with some residual
temporal disorganization, as well. These residual or hangover effects
may mimic the withdrawal syndrome, both qualitatively and
quantitatively, which is expressed after chronic marijuana exposure.
This acute hangover may reflect a true acute withdrawal syndrome
similar to that experienced from high acute alcohol intake. The
presence of an acute withdrawal syndrome after drug administration has
been suggested to represent a physiological compensatory rebound by
which chronic administration of the drug will eventually potentiate and
produce dependence and the potential for continued abuse (Gauvin, Cheng
& Holloway, 1993).
Crowley et al. (1998) screened marijuana users for DSM-IIIR
dependence criteria. Of the 165 males and 64 female patients that met
the criteria, 82.1% were found to have co-morbid conduct disorders;
17.5% had major depression; and 14.8% had a diagnosis of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. These results also showed that most
patients claimed to have “serious problems” from cannabis use. The
data also indicated that for adolescents with conduct problems,
cannabis use was not benign, and that the drug served as a potent
reinforcer for further cannabis usage, producing dependence and
withdrawal.
Kelly & Jones (1992) quantified concentrations of THC and its
metabolites in both plasma and urine after a 5 mg intravenous dose of
THC was administered to frequent and infrequent marijuana smokers. The
frequent smokers were users who smoked marijuana almost daily for at
least two years. The infrequent smokers were users who smoked marijuana
no more than two to three times per month but had done so for at least
two years. Pharmacokinetic parameters after intravenously administered
THC revealed no significant differences between frequent and infrequent
marijuana users on area under the time-effect curve (AUC), volume of
distribution, elimination half-lives of parent THC and metabolites in
plasma and urine. There were also no group differences in metabolic or
renal clearances. The authors concluded that there was no evidence for
metabolic or dispositional tolerance between the two groups of
subjects. Kelly and Jones also reported that tolerance was not evident
in heart rate, diastolic blood pressure, skin temperature, and the
degree of psychological “high” from the i.v. administration of THC.
In two separate reports, Haney et al. have recently described
abstinence symptoms of an acute withdrawal syndrome following high (30
mg q.i.d.) and low (20 mg q.i.d) dose administrations of oral THC
(Haney et al., 1999a) and following 5 puffs of high (3.1%) and low
(1.8%) THC-containing smoked marijuana cigarettes (Haney et al.,
1999b). Abstinence from oral THC increased ratings of “anxious”,
“depressed”, and “irritable”, and decreased the reported quantity
and quality of sleep and decreased food intake by 20-30% compared to
baseline. Abstinence from as low as 5 controlled puffs of active
marijuana smoking increased ratings of “anxious”, “irritable” and
“stomach pain”, and
[[Page 20056]]
significantly decreased food intake. The 5 controlled puffs of 5 second
duration each were drawn from 2 separate marijuana cigarettes (3 puffs
from one, 2 puffs from the other. The smoke was held for 40 seconds and
then exhaled. All subjects reported significant increases on subjective
measures of “high”, “good drug effect”, and “stimulated”, as well
as “mellow”, “content”, and “friendly” as a result of this
limited and controlled draw of THC. Both of these studies have
delineated a withdrawal syndrome from concentrations of THC
significantly lower than those reported in any other previous study
and, for the first time, clearly identified a marijuana withdrawal
syndrome detected at low levels of THC exposure that do not produce
tolerance. The abstinence syndrome was not limited to subjective state
changes but was also quantified using a cognitive/memory test battery.
In a related study, Khouri et al (1999) found that long-term heavy
marijuana users became more aggressive during abstinence from marijuana
than did former or infrequent users. Previous dependence studies have
relied largely on patients’ subjective reports of a range of symptoms.
Khouri et al. examined a single symptom–aggression. The authors
concluded that marijuana abstinence is associated with unpleasant
behavioral symptoms that may contribute to continued marijuana use.
Kouri & Pope (2000) examined three groups of marijuana users during
a 28-day supervised abstinence period. Current marijuana users
experienced significant increases in anxiety, irritability, physical
tension, and physical symptoms and decreases in mood and appetite
during marijuana withdrawal. These symptoms were most pronounced during
the initial 10 days of abstinence, bust some were present for the
entire 28-day withdrawal period. The findings from this study reveal
that chronic heavy users of marijuana experience a number of withdrawal
symptoms during abstinence and clearly demonstrate a “marijuana
dependence syndrome” in humans.
These data suggest that dependence on THC may in fact be an
important consequence of repeated, daily exposure to cannabinoids and
that daily marijuana use may be maintained, at least in part, by the
alleviation of abstinence symptoms. Relevant to the present petition,
the Haney et al. study is the first report demonstrating this syndrome
with extremely low concentrations of THC.
Results of THC Dose Comparison Studies
There are reports in the scientific literature that evaluated dose-
related subjective and reinforcing effects of Cannabis sativa in
humans. These studies have assessed the subjective and reinforcing
effects of cannabis cigarettes containing different potencies of THC
and/or which have manipulated the THC dose by varying the volume of THC
smoke inhaled (Azorlosa et al., 1992; Lukas et al., 1995; Chait et al.,
1988; Chait and Burke, 1994; Kelly et al., 1993).
Chait et al. (1988) studied the discriminative stimulus effects of
smoked marijuana cigarettes containing THC contents of 0%, 0.9%, 1.4%,
2.7%. Marijuana smokers were trained to discriminate smoked marijuana
from placebo using 4 puffs of a 2.7%-THC cigarettes. Subjective ratings
of “high”, and physiological measures (i.e., heart rate) were
significantly and dose-dependently increased after smoking the 0.9%,
1.4%, 2.7%.
Marijuana cigarettes containing 1.4% THC completely substituted for
2.7%-THC on drug identification tasks, however, 0.9%-THC did not. The
authors found that the onset of discriminative stimulus effects was
within 90 seconds after smoking began (after the first two puffs).
Since the 1.4%-THC cigarette substituted for 2-puffs of the 2.7%-THC
cigarette, the authors estimate that an inhaled dose of THC as low as 3
mg can produce discriminable subjective effects.
Similarly, Lukas et al. (1995) reported that marijuana cigarettes
containing either 1.26% or 2.53% THC produced significant and dose-
dependent increases in level of intoxication and euphoria in male
occasional marijuana smokers. Four of the six subjects that smoked the
1.26%-THC cigarette reported marijuana effects and 75% of these
subjects reported euphoria. All six of the subjects that smoked 2.53%
THC reported marijuana effects and euphoria. Peak levels of self-
reported intoxication occurred at 15 and 30 minutes after smoking and
returned to control levels by 90-105 minutes. There was no difference
between latency to or duration of euphoria after smoking either the
1.26% or 2.53% THC cigarettes. The higher dose-marijuana cigarette
produced a more rapid onset and longer duration of action than the
lower dose marijuana cigarette (1.26% THC). Plasma THC levels peaked 5-
10 minutes after smoking began; the average peak level attained after
the low- and high-dose marijuana cigarette was 36 and 69 ng/ml
respectively.
In order to determine marijuana dose-effects on subjective and
performance measures over a wide dose range, Azorlosa et al. (1992)
evaluated the effects of 4, 10, or 25 puffs from marijuana cigarettes
containing 1.75 or 3.55% THC in seven male moderate users of marijuana.
Orderly dose-response curves were produced for subjective drug effects,
heart rate, and plasma concentration, as a function of THC content and
number of puffs. After smoking the 1.75% THC cigarette, maximal plasma
THC levels were 57 ng/ml immediately after smoking, 18.3 ng/ml 15
minutes after smoking, 10.3 ng/ml 30 minutes after smoking, and 7.7 ng/
ml 45 minutes after smoking.
The study also showed that subjects could smoke more of the low THC
cigarette to produce effects that were similar to the high THC dose
cigarette (Azorlosa et al., 1992). There were nearly identical THC
levels produced by 10-puff low-THC cigarette (98.6 ng/ml) and 4-puff
high THC cigarette (89.4 ng/ml). Similarly, the subjective effects
ratings, including high, stoned, impaired, confused, clear-headed and
sluggish, produced under the 10 puff low- and high-THC and 25 puff low-
THC conditions did not differ significantly from each other.
As with most drugs of abuse, higher doses of marijuana are
preferred over lower dose. Although not preferred, these lower doses
still produce cannabimimetic effects. Twelve regular marijuana smokers
participated in a study designed to determine the preference of a low
potency (0.64%-THC) vs. a high potency (1.95%-THC) marijuana cigarette
(Chait and Burke, 1994). The subjects first sampled the marijuana of
two different potencies in one session, then chose which potency and
how much to smoke. During sampling sessions, there were significant
dose-dependent increases in heart rate and subjective effects,
including ratings of peak “high”, strength of drug effects,
stimulated, and drug liking. During choice sessions, the higher dose
marijuana was chosen over the lower dose marijuana on 87.5% of
occasions. Not surprising, there was a significant positive correlation
between the total number of cigarettes smoked and the ratings of
subjective effects, strength of drug effect, drug “liking”, expired
air carbon monoxide, and heart rate increases. The authors state it is
not necessary valid to assume that the preference observed in the
present study for the high-potency marijuana was due to greater CNS
effects from its higher THC content. The present study found that the
low- and high-potency marijuana cigarettes also differ on
[[Page 20057]]
several sensory dimensions; the high-potency THC was found to be
reported as “fresher” and “hotter”. Other studies found that
marijuana cigarettes containing different THC contents varied in
sensory dimensions (cf., Chait et al., 1988; Nemeth-Coslett et al.,
1986).
As summarized by Martin & Hall for the United Nations only a small
amount of cannabis (e.g. 2-3 mg of available THC) is required to
produce a brief pleasurable high for the occasional user and a single
joint may be sufficient for two or three individuals. Using these data
and those of Harder & Reitbroch (1997, above), a one gram cigarette
containing 1% THC containing cannabis, would contain 10 mg of THC–a
dose well capable of producing a social high.
Carlini et al. (1974) examined 33 subjects who smoked marijuana
cigarettes with different ratios of constituent cannabinoids. The plant
containing 0.82% THC produced larger than expected results based on the
estimates from the THC content.
Smoking a 250 mg cigarette containing 5.0 mg of
9-THC induced more reactions graded 3 and 4 than 10
or 20 mg of 9-THC. It was further observed that the
psychological effects (subjective “high”) started around 10 min after
the end of the inhalation, and reached a maximum 20 to 30 min later,
subsiding within 1 to 3 hrs. The peak of psychological disturbances,
therefore, did not coincide in time with the peak of pulse rate
effects. Carlini et al., suggested that other constituents of the
marijuana were interacting synergistically with the THC to potentiate
the subjective response induced by the smoking of the cigarette.
Karniol and colleagues (1973, 1974) have clearly demonstrated that
cannabidiol (CBD) blocks some of the effects induced by THC, such as
increased pulse rates and disturbed time perception. More importantly,
CBD blocked some of the psychological effects of THC, but not by
altering the quantitative or intensity of the psychological reactions.
CBD seemed better able to block the aversive effects of THC. CBD
changed the symptoms reported by the subjects in such a way that the
anxiety component produced by THC administration was actually reduced.
The animal subjects of one study showed greater analgesia scores with a
CBD+THC combination (1973) and the human subjects from the other study
(1974) showed less anxiety and panic but reported more pleasurable
effects. CBD may be best seen as an “entourage” compound (Mechoulam,
Fride, DiMarzo, 1998) which is administered along with THC and results
in a functional potentiation of THC’s behavioral and subjective
effects. This potentiation can be in both the intensity and/or duration
of the high induced by marijuana. According to Paris & Nahas (1984) the
CBD:THC ratio in industrial or fiber type hemp is 2:1. Relevant to the
current petition, the CBD:THC ratio producing the greatest increase in
euphoria in the Karniol, et al. studies was 2:1 (60:30 mg).
Jones & Pertwee (1972) were first to report that the presence of
cannabidiol inhibited the metabolism of THC and its active metabolite.
These data were soon replicated by Nilsson et al., (1973). Bronheim et
al., (1995) examined the effects of CBD on the pharmacokinetic profile
of THC content in both blood and brains of mice. CBD pretreatments
produced a modest elevation in THC-blood levels; area under the
kinetics curve of THC was increased by 50% as a function of decreased
clearance. CBD pretreatments also modestly increased the
Cmax, AUC, and half-life of the major THC metabolites in the
blood. The THC kinetics function showed a 7- to 15-fold increase in the
area under the curve, a 2- to 4-fold increase in the half-life, as well
as the tmax. CBD pretreatments resulted in large increases
in area under the curves and half-lives of all the THC metabolites in
the mice brains. The inhibition of the metabolism of THC and its
psychoactive metabolites by CBD may underlie the potentiation in the
subjective effects of THC by CBD in humans.
In addition to THC, hemp material contains a variety of other
substances (e.g., Hollister, 1974), including other cannabinoids such
as cannabidiol (CBD) and cannabinol (CBN). One comprehensive review
described the activities of 300 cannabinoid compound in preclinical
models (Razdan, 1986). Since CBD is always present in preparations of
cannabis, it may represent a high CBD:THC ratio in the case of low THC
cannabis. Therefore, it is important to understand the interactions of
cannabidiol and 9-THC.
Structure-activity studies of cannabinoid compounds characterized
cannabidiol in relationship to 9-THC and other
cannabinoids (Martin et al., 1981; Little et al., 1988). These and
other studies have found that cannabidiol was inactive and did not
produce neuropharmacological effects or discriminative stimulus,
subjective effects and behavioral effects predictive of psychoactive
subjective effects (Howlett, 1987; Howlett et al., 1992; c.f., Hiltunen
and Jarbe, 1986; Perez-Reyes et al., 1973; Zuardi et al., 1982; Karniol
et al., 1974).
Other studies have reported that cannabidiol has cannabinoid
properties, including anticonvulsant effects in animal and human models
(Consroe et al., 1981; Carlini & Cunha, 1981; Doyle and Spence, 1995),
hypnotic effects (Monti, 1977), anxiolytic effects (Musty, 1984;
Onaivi, Geen, & Martin, 1990; Guimarares et al., 1990; 1994) and rate-
decreasing effects on operant behavior (Hiltunen et al., 1988).
Experiments with cannabidiol in combination with THC have found
that certain behavioral responses induced by THC (i.e., operant,
schedule-controlled responding) were attenuated by cannabidiol (Borgen
and Davis, 1974; Brady and Balster, 1980; Consroe et al., 1977; Dalton
et al., 1976; Kraniol and Carlini, 1973; Karniol et al., 1974; Welburn
et al., 1976; Zuardi and Karniol, 1983; Zuardi et al., 1981, 1982;
Hiltunen et al., 1988). However, other affects produced by THC are
augmented or prolonged by the combined administration of CBD and THC or
marijuana extract (Chesher and Jackson, 1974; Hine et al., 1975a,b;
Fernandes et al., 1974; Karniol and Carlini, 1973; Musty and Sands,
1978; Zuardi and Karniol, 1983; Zuardi et al., 1984). Still other
studies did not report any behavioral interaction between the CBD and
THC (Bird et al., 1980; Browne and Weissman, 1981; Hollister and
Gillespie, 1975; Jarbe and Henricksson, 1974; Jarbe et al., 1977;
Mechoulam et al., 1970; Sanders et al., 1979; Ten Ham and DeLong,
1975).
A study to characterize the interaction between CBD and THC was
conducted using preclinical drug discrimination procedures. Rats and
pigeons trained to discriminate the presence or absence of THC, and
tested with CBD administered alone and in combinations with THC
(Hiltunen and Jarbe, 1986).
Specifically, in rats trained to discriminate 3.0 mg/kg, i.p. THC,
CBD (30.0 mg/kg) was administered alone and in combination with THC
(0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg, i.p.). In pigeons trained to discriminate 0.56 mg/
kg, i.m. THC, CBD (17.5 mg/kg) was administered alone and in
combination with THC (0.1, 0.3, and 0.56 mg/kg, i.m.). CBD prolonged
the discriminative stimulus effects of THC in rats, but did not change
the time-effect curve for THC in pigeons. In pigeons, the
administration of CBD did not produce any differential effect under a
fixed ratio schedule of reinforcement (Hiltunen and Jarbe, 1986).
These data suggest that CBD may somehow augment or prolong the
actions of THC in rats and had no effect in pigeons. In the present
study, the CBD/THC ratios ranged from 30:1 to 100:1 in rats and
enhanced the stimulus
[[Page 20058]]
effects of THC. However, similar CBD/THC ratios in pigeons (31:1, 58:1
and 175:1) did not result in any changes to THC’s discriminative
stimulus or response rate effects (Hiltunen and Jarbe, 1986).
It should be noted that cannabidiol can be easily converted to
delta-9- and delta-8-tetrahydrocannabinol. Even industrial hemp plant
material (leaves), containing high concentrations of CBD, can be
treated in clandestine laboratories to convert the CBD to delta-9-
tetrahydrocannabinol (Mechoulam, 1973) converting a supposedly
innocuous weed into a potent smoke product.
In conclusion, the “entourage” compound, cannabidiol, does
contribute to all of the effects ascribed to THC, however it also
appears to lack cannabimimetic properties. However, there is no
credible scientific evidence that CBD is a pharmacological antagonist
at the cannabinoid receptor (Howlett, Evans, & Houston, 1992). There is
clear evidence that CBD can functionally antagonize some of the
aversive effects of THC (Dewey, 1986). The data from the scientific
literature cited above, clearly demonstrate the ability of CBD to
modify some very specific effects of THC. Most importantly, relative to
the euphorigenic effects of THC (which contributes to its abuse
liability), CBD appears to potentiate the psychological or subjective
effects of THC by potentiating the blood and brain THC and 11-OH-THC
levels and by functionally blocking the aversive (anxiety-like)
properties of THC.
Abuse Liability Summary
Preclinical and clinical experimental data demonstrate that
marijuana and “9-THC have similar abuse
liabilities (i.e., drug discrimination, self-administration, subjective
effects). Both preclinical and clinical studies show that
discontinuation of either marijuana or “9-THC
administration produces a mild withdrawal syndrome. The effects of THC
are dose-dependent and several studies have found that low-potency THC
is behaviorally active and can produce cannabimimetic-like subjective
and physiological effects.
Actual Abuse
There are dozens of data collection and reporting systems that are
useful for monitoring the United States’ problem with abuse of licit
and illicit substances. These data collection and reporting systems
provide quantitative data on many factors related to abuse of a
particular substance, including incidence, pattern, consequence and
profile of the abuser of specific substances (cf., Larsen et al.,
1995).
Evidence of actual abuse is defined by episodes/mentions in the
databases indicative of abuse/dependence. Some of the databases that
are utilized by DEA to provide data relevant to actual abuse of a
substance include the Drug Abuse Warning Network (DAWN), National
Household Survey on Drug Abuse, Monitoring the Future survey, FDA’s
Spontaneous Adverse Events Reports, the American Association of Poison
Control Centers database and reports of the Community Epidemiology Work
Group (CEWG).
Drug trafficking and diversion data provide strong evidence that a
drug or other substance is being abused. In order to determine the
pattern, incidence, and consequences of abuse and the demographics of
abusers of a particular substance to be controlled, DEA relies on data
collected from a number of sources, including the United States
government as well as state and local law enforcement groups.
Information from these sources often provides a first indication of an
emerging pattern of abuse of a particular drug or substance, and when
taken together with other data sources provide strong evidence that can
be used in determining a substance’s placement in the schedules listed
in the CSA.
The evidence from epidemiological studies conclude that marijuana
use alone and in combination with other illicit drugs is increasing.
The most recent “Monitoring the Future Study”, documented increases
in lifetime, annual and current (within the past 30 days) and daily use
of marijuana by eighth and tenth graders; this increasing trend began
in the early 1990’s.
Similarly, according the NIDA’s “National Household Survey”,
marijuana use is increasing with the greatest increase among the
younger age groups (12-17 years of age). The frequency of marijuana use
in the past year increases significantly among 12-17 year olds. This
survey also found that youths who used marijuana at least once in their
lives were more likely to engage in violent or other antisocial
behaviors.
Marijuana is the most readily available illicit drug in the United
States. Cannabis is cultivated in remote locations and frequently on
public lands. Major domestic outdoor cannabis cultivation areas are
found in California, Hawaii, Kentucky, New York and Tennessee.
Significant quantities of marijuana were seized from indoor cultivation
operations; there were 3,532 seizures in 1996 compared to 3,348 seized
in 1995. Mexico is the major source of foreign marijuana, along with
lesser amounts from Colombia and Jamaica (NNICC, 1996).
Domestically, marijuana is distributed by groups or individuals,
ranging from large sophisticated organizations with controlled
cultivation and interstate trafficking, to small independent
traffickers at the local level.
(2) Scientific Evidence of Its Pharmacological Effects, If Known
Cannabis sativa is unique in that it is the only botanical source
of the terpenophenolic substances referred to as cannabinoids which are
responsible for the psychoactive effects of Cannabis. There are roughly
60 different cannabinoids found in Cannabis (Nahas, 1984; Murphy &
Bartke, 1992; Agurell, Dewey & Willette, 1984) but the psychoactive
properties of Cannabis are attributed to one or two of the major
cannabinoid substances, namely delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and delta-
8-tetrahydrocannabinol. In fresh, carefully dried marijuana, up to 95%
of their cannabinoids are present as (-)-delta-9-(trans)-
tetrahydrocannabinol carboxylic acid (Nahas, 1984; Murphy & Bartke,
1992; Agurell, Dewey & Willette, 1984). The acid form is not
psychoactive, but is readily decarboxylated upon heating to yield
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (neutral form). Therefore, plant material
could be very high in its “pro-drug” acid form and very low in
neutral form but still be very potent when smoked.
There are two primary factors that influence THC content: genetic
predisposition and environmental influences. Genetic factors are
considered predominant in determining cannabinoid content, although,
fluctuations in weather conditions have greatly enhanced or diminished
the THC content.
Paris & Nahas (1984) have admonished that marijuana is not a single
uniform plant like many of those encountered in nature, but a rather
deceptive weed with several hundred variants. The intoxicating
substances prepared from Cannabis vary considerably in potency
according to the varying mixtures of different parts of the plant, and
according to the techniques of fabrication. According to Paris & Nahas,
this basic botanical fact has been overlooked by physicians and
educators, who have written about marijuana as a simple, single
substance, which uniformly yields a low concentration of a single
intoxicant. In addition to changes due to its own genetic plasticity,
marijuana has been modified throughout the ages by environmental
factors and human manipulations, and is not yet a
[[Page 20059]]
stabilized botanical species (Paris & Nahas, 1984).
According to Paris & Nahas (1984) the terminology used by Fetterman
et al. (1970, 1971) is somewhat misleading, especially with respect to
their contention that environmental factors, including climate, are not
as important as heredity in determining the cannabinoid content of
cutigens. The analyses of Fetterman et al., (1970) were performed
according to the technique by Doorenbos et al., (1971) on plant
materials from variants that had been cut at the stem beneath the
lowest leaves and air-dried. Seeds, bracts, flowers, leaves and small
stems were then stripped from the plant. Most of the small stems were
removed by a 10-mesh screen, and the seeds were eliminated with a
mechanical seed separator. This preparation of marijuana contains less
seed and stem than most of the illicit material available in the United
States. Cannabinoids were then extracted from the plant material and
analyzed by standard techniques.
Other systems of separating Cannabis into drug, intermediate and
non-drug type have been developed. These are typically determined by
chemical analyses based upon the method described by Doorenbos (1971)
which utilizes manicured portions of the Cannabis plant only in
determining percent concentration.
Cannabis sativa has been referred to as a widely distributed and
unstabilized species. Cannabis exhibits extreme polymorphism (ability
to alter, change) in different varieties, dependent upon many factors.
For example, there are at least twenty strains which are cultivated for
fiber. There have been many attempts to classify Cannabis as a function
of intoxicant properties or fiber properties. Such classification
efforts are dependent upon the age of the sample. And there is no
totally reliable classification system based on a single chemical
analysis. The plasticity of the genus has prevented the development of
such a system (Turner et al. 1980a,b).
In a study where twelve strains of Cannabis were grown out of doors
in Southern England (Fairbairn and Liebmann, 1974, Fairbairn et al.,
1971), the following were determined:
- Warm climate are not necessary for high THC content.
- There is considerable THC content variation within and between
plants.
- Quantitative results of tetrahydrocannabinol concentration (THC)
are highly dependent upon the specific plant part sampled, the stage of
growth and the size of sample.
- Certain strains of Cannabis can be THC or cannabidiol (CBD) rich
which does not seem to be dependent upon environmental conditions.
- However, growing the same strain of Cannabis under different
lighting conditions can produce plants that range from 2.4 to 4.42% THC
concentration (based upon an analysis of the upper leaves). And
finally,
- THC concentration are dramatically higher on dried flowering or
vegetative tops of the plants relative to middle or lower portions.
In a similar study on the characterization of Cannabis accessions
with regard to cannabinoid content, vis-a-vis other plant characters
(deMeijer, 1992), it was determined that:
- There exists considerable variation within and among accessions
for cannabinoid content;
- Mean cannabinoid content is strongly affected by year of
cultivation;
- There is no strict relationship between chemical and non-
chemical traits; and,
- It is uncommon, but some accessions combine high bark fiber
content and considerable psychoactive potency.
In 1993 de Meijer reported the results of a government
(Netherlands) funded industrial hemp project designed to investigate
the stem quality, yield, and a comparative analysis to wood fibers.
deMeijer found that the commercial grade industrial hemp seeds,
germplasms derived from 0.3% THC chemovars, demonstrated a significant
variation in the average THC content which ranged from 0.06 to 1.77% in
the female dry leaf matter. deMeijer concluded by stating,
Although high bark fiber content does not necessarily exclude
high THC content, most fiber cultivars have very low THC content and
thus possess no psychoactive potency
While the data from his own study refutes these conclusions he does
conclude that the industrial hemp plant does not preclude high THC
content.
A review of these and other studies in the scientific literature,
indicate that THC concentrations vary within portions of the Cannabis
plant (Hanus et al., 1989, 1975). In some studies, the concentration of
THC can increase as much as 100% from leafy to flowering portions of
the same plant. THC concentrations are known to be elevated on the
upper portions of the plant. In a study published by Fairbairn and
Liebmann, (1974) there was considerable variations between the
flowering tops (bracts, flowers, immature fruits at the ends of shoots)
and leafy portions of some specimens. THC content decreases with age
and length of leaves (Paris & Nahas, 1984, p 25). The lower, more
developed leaves have a low cannabinoid content and the top leaves have
a high cannabinoid content, especially when they are associated with
the bracts of the plant. Cannabinoids are localized in the upper third
of the “stalk” and in the flowers. Therefore, the THC content of
specific portions of a plant, which on a whole plant basis did not
exceed 1%, could significantly exceed this threshold. Very few
marijuana users actually “smoke” the leaves. It is the colas or the
flowering portions of the plants which are utilized and these are
exactly the portions of the plant which would be expected to have the
highest concentration of THC.
It is clearly recognized that Cannabis presents a high degree of
genetic plasticity which results in extreme polymorphism in its
different varieties. The hemp first grown in the United States for
fiber was of European origin. The type basic to modern American fiber
production, known as Kentucky, came originally from China. In Europe,
there are five to six varieties with one considered “exceptional”–
the Kymington. The plasticity of the European fiber variety has been
clearly shown (Bouquet, 1951; Hamilton, 1912, 1915). European cultigens
planted in dry, warm areas of Egypt to supply fiber for rope-making
were found to produce, within several generations, plants with high
psycho-active ingredients and very little fiber. Cannabis sativa’s
botanical and chemical characteristics change markedly as a result of
environmental factors and human manipulation. Doorenbos et al., (1971)
cultivated a Mexican and Turkish variant in Mississippi for three
consecutive generations. During that period, the 9-
THC content did not change in the Mexican variant but increased in the
Turkish variant. In the more controlled environment of a phytotron
(light, humidity, and nutrition controlled), Braut-Boucher (1978),
Braut-Boucher & Petiard (1981), Braut-Boucher, Paris, & Cosson (1977)
and Paris et al., (1975) found that the cannabinoid concentrations rose
over a similar three year period. The concentrations rose more sharply
in cool environments (22-12 deg.C: day-night) than in warm environments
(32-12 deg.C). Some authors have hypothesized that immediate
environmentally caused changes are individual plant reactions, whereas
the progressive changes over generations are linked with whole
populations and constitute a true natural selection. Whether this
evolution is caused by a change of genetic equilibrium (caused by the
environment), or by a
[[Page 20060]]
modification of the genetic capacity (over time), is impossible to say
(Paris & Nahas, 1984).
In 1974 through 1976 the University of Mississippi cultivated 7
variants of 12 Cannabis plants discovered and collected in 1973 from
different areas of Mexico. Cannabinoid content was analyzed weekly
during the cultivation period. Turner, Elsohly, Lewis, Lopez-Santibanez
& Carranza (1982) summarized their findings as follows:
In 1974, vegetative plants of ME-H, ME-K, ME-L, ME-N and ME-O,
at 13 weeks of age had higher 9-THC content that
at weeks 12 and 14. They showed minimum 9-THC
content at week 15. For the most part, 1974 staminate and pistillate
plants grown in Mississippi produced a low 9-THC
concentration * * *.
In all variants, the average 9-THC was higher
in 1976 than in 1974. Also, a greater fluctuation of
9-THC was observed in 1976 than in 1974.
These results further establish that Cannabis Sativa L. is not a
stable hybrid plant, but rather, represents characteristics more
similar to an unstable weed.
Marijuana chemistry is complex and cannot be simplified or
extrapolated from any one or two “active compounds”. As early as 1974
this fact was recognized by the United Nations Division on Narcotic
Drugs (UN Doc, 1974). As highlighted by Turner (1980), the chemistry of
THC is not the chemistry of marijuana and the pharmacology of marijuana
is not the pharmacology of THC. Recent findings do suggest that the
interactions between cannabinoids is one of many critical factors in
the analysis of the psychopharmacology of marijuana.
According to Jones (1980), because of exposure to a wide range of
plant material and the cultural labeling (almost like advertising) of
much of the marijuana experience, marijuana users are particularly
subject to the effects of nonpharmacological variables that alter the
subjective response to marijuana intoxication (Jones 1971, 1980;
Cappell & Pliner, 1974; Becker 1967). As reviewed by Jones (1971), a
number of studies suggest that experienced marijuana users are more
subject to “placebo reactions’; that is, a degree of intoxication
disproportionate to the THC content of the material. This seems
particularly true if the individuals are exposed to low potency
marijuana (1.0% THC). Jones believes that this is a result of
experience and practice at recognizing minimal physiologic cues
together with the smell, taste and other sensations associated with
smoking a marijuana cigarette (Jones 1980, 1971). Becker 1967 and
Cappell & Pliner (1974) have described a number of psychological
factors (expectancy, social setting, etc.) that appear to
synergistically interact to help generate the subjective experiences
engendered by marijuana smoking.
Domino, Rennick, & Pearl (1976) administered THC injected into
tobacco cigarettes to male volunteers. Similar to findings described by
Isbell et al., (1967) they report that 50 g of THC into the
cigarettes produced a “social high”, while 250 g/kg was
“hallucinogenic”. Taking 80 kg as the mean weight of their subjects
the authors concluded that a 4.0 mg total THC dose produced a “social
high”; a hallucinogenic dose was 20 mg total THC by inhalation. A
standard 1g cigarette of 1% THC fibre-type hemp provides 10 mg of THC.
Even allowing for a 50% loss of THC from sidestream smoke and
pyrolysis, smoking this cigarette provides more than enough THC to
produce a “social high”.
In 1968 Weil, Norman, & Nelsen described a set of studies examining
the physiological and psychological aspects of smoked marijuana. The
first batch of Mexican grown marijuana used in the study was found to
contain only 0.3% THC by weight. The potency of this product was
considered to be “low” by the experimenters on the basis of the doses
needed to produce symptoms of intoxication in the chronic users. This
low potency marijuana was able to produce a “high”, but only with two
1 gram cigarettes. A second batch was used in later studies. Weil,
Norman, & Nelsen report that marijuana assayed at 0.9% THC (a quantity
slightly less than the 1% THC limit set forth by the petitioners) was
rated by the chronic users in the study to be “good, average”
marijuana, neither exceptionally strong nor exceptionally weak compared
to the usual supplies. Users consistently reported symptoms of
intoxication after smoking about 0.5 grams of the 0.9% THC containing
marijuana (half a joint). With the high dose of marijuana (2.0 grams of
0.9% THC containing marijuana) all chronic users became “high” by
their own accounts and in the judgment of experimenters who had
observed many persons under the influence of marijuana.
Agurell & Leander (1971) examined the physiological and
psychological effects of low THC-containing cannabis in experienced
users. They reported that 14-29% of the cannabinoid content of the
cigarette was transferred to the main stream smoke. Based on
qualitative and quantitative analyses, Agurell & Leander demonstrated
that as little as 3-5 mg of THC was needed to be absorbed by the lung
in order to produce a “normal biological high”. Further, they found
that as little as 1 mg of absorbed THC was discriminable by all of
their chronic user subjects.
In 1982, Barnett, Chiang, Perez-Reyes, & Owens had six subjects
smoke a 1% THC-containing (industrial hemp, as defined by the
petitioner) marijuana cigarette. Significant heart rate and subjective
measures of “high” were measured for 2 hours after each cigarette.
In 1971 Jones reported on the wide variability in THC
concentrations found in street samples:
Specimens gathered in the midwestern United States contained
only 0.1–0.5% THC. Thirty specimens selected from seized samples in
the Bureau of Narcotics and Dangerous Drugs Laboratory in San
Francisco all contained less than 1% THC. Samples from the State of
California Bureau of Narcotic enforcement analyzed in our laboratory
contained as little as 0.1% THC and a maximum of 0.9% * * * In a
survey done in Ontario, Canada, Marshman and Gibbons found that of
36 samples alleged to be marijuana with high cannabinoid content,
34% contained no marijuana at all, and much of the rest was cut with
other plant substances. A generous assumption is that marijuana
generally available in the United States averages about 1.0% THC.
It must be acknowledged that the THC content of domestically grown
and imported marijuana has increased since these reports. However, the
description by Weil, Zinberg & Nelson (1968), Agurell & Leander (1971),
Jones (1971) and Barnett et al. (1982) highlight the historical
importance of low THC concentrations contained in marijuana which
provided the basis for the marijuana culture that developed in the
1970s. The incident described by Jones was not an isolated case of the
inadvertent misrepresentation of the THC content of marijuana extracts.
Caldwell et al., (1969) found that the NIMH-supplied marijuana that
they reported to have contained 1.3% THC was analyzed by two
independent laboratories and found to contain as little as 0.2 to 0.5%
THC. Similarly, according to Paton & Pertwee (1973) the THC content of
material used by Clark & Nakashima (1968), Weil et al., (1968), Weil &
Zinberg (1969), and Crancer et al., (1969) must be expected to be one-
third to one-sixth less than stated. This means that the positive
results of all of these studies were the result of a surprisingly low
THC-containing (1.0%) marijuana. The early scientific data on the
subjective effects of marijuana were generated with these samples by
experienced smokers smoking material in this potency range. These
experienced marijuana smokers were reporting that these marijuana
[[Page 20061]]
samples were of “average quality” (Mechoulam, 1973).
In an early study, Jones (1971) utilized 1 gram of plant material
with a THC concentration of 0.9% (9 mg of THC). Experienced marijuana
smokers were asked to freely smoke marijuana cigarettes for 10 minutes.
The smoking topography of the smokers widely varied and was not
controlled in this set of experiments. Subjects were asked to smoke the
entire cigarette. Subjective state was measured by asking the subjects
to make global estimates of his degree of intoxication on a 0-100
scale. A score of 0 was defined as “sober” and a score of 100 as the
most intoxicated or most “stoned” they had ever been in any social
situation. At the end of the session (about 3 hrs), the subject also
filled out a 272-item symptom checklist (SDEQ: subjective drug effects
questionnaire) which taps some of the more unusual emotional,
perceptual and cognitive effects produced by psychoactive drugs. The
mean potency rating was 61 for the marijuana containing only 9 mg of
THC. There was a tremendous range in the rating made by individual
smokers. Jones concluded that the smokers may obtain intermittent
reinforcement from THC but where much of the behavior and subsequent
response is maintained by “conditioned reinforcers” such as the whole
ritual of lighting up, the associated stimuli of smell, taste, visual
stimuli and so on.
Manno, Kiplinger, Haine, Bennett, & Forney (1970) asked subjects to
smoke an entire 1 gram cigarette containing 1% THC (10 mg; low
potency). The subjects were told to take 2 to 4 seconds to inhale and
to hold the draw for 30 to 60 seconds. The expired smoke was collected
and analyzed for THC content, as well. During the experiment the
subjects smoked the entire cigarette; in all cases, less than 0.5 mg of
THC remained in the residue of each cigarette. Manno et al. reported
that the quantity of THC or other cannabinols present in a marijuana
cigarette was not a reliable indicator of the amount of cannabinols
that were delivered in the smoke of the cigarette. Controlled smoking
experiments through a manufactured smoking machine demonstrated that
approximately 50% of the \9\-THC originally present in the
cigarette was delivered unchanged in the smoke. Manno et al. concluded
that a dose of approximately 5 mg of \9\-THC was delivered
which was estimated to be an administered dose in the range of 50 to 75
g per kilogram. These low potency marijuana cigarettes
produced significant motor and mental performance measures on the
pursuit meter test, delayed auditory feedback, verbal output, reverse
reading, reverse counting, progressive counting, simple addition,
subtraction, addition +7, subtract +7, and color differentiation. These
low potency cigarettes also produced significant pulse rate increases
and significant increases on a somatic symptoms checklist. Unsolicited
verbal comments from the subjects verified that the subjects were
“high” on these low potency marijuana cigarettes.
Kiplinger, Manno, Rodda, Forney, Haine, Ease, & Richards (1971)
conducted a randomized block, double-blind study designed to establish
a dose-response analysis of the THC content in marijuana using a
variety of behavioral and subjective effects measures. Marijuana
cigarettes were manufactured to deliver doses of 0, 6.25, 12.5, 25, and
50 g/kg of \9\-THC. Based on an average 70 kg man,
the total delivered doses of THC were 0, 0.43, 0.875, 1.75, and 3.5 mg.
Based on the assumption of a 50% loss of THC from pyrolysis and
sidestream smoke these doses would be equivalent to smoking cigarettes
containing 0, 0.08%, 0.16%, 0.3%, and 0.7% THC containing hemp. The
lower concentrations of THC were used because these doses are found in
the weaker “hemp” or fiber type marijuana commonly grown in the
United States. All doses of THC, including the two lowest doses,
increased the subjective ratings on both the ARCI and Cornell Medical
Indexes, produced heart-rate increases, increased motoric decrements in
pursuit meter, and produced decrements in mental performance using the
delayed auditory feedback test. Most importantly, 80% of subjects
correctly identified the lowest dose (6.25 g/kg; 0.43 mg THC)
as active marijuana. The authors suggested that even lower doses might
have measurable effects. Holtzman (1971) has suggested that one of the
best predictors of a drug’s abuse liability is the identification of
the substance as “drug-like” by experienced drug users. The
identification of the lowest dose of marijuana in the Kiplinger et al.
and the other studies, discussed above, clearly suggests that
industrial “fiber-type” marijuana has abuse potential.
Many of the studies examining the behavioral effects of marijuana
in animals have chosen to administer THC because of the difficulties in
controlling and administering exact doses within and between subjects
when using pyrolyzed forms of marijuana to animals. Accurate small-
animal smoke delivery systems are not yet available. The lack of water
solubility of \9\-THC has made its administration and
absorption a difficult problem for pharmacologists. Many different
methods for suspending, solubilizing, or emulsifying \9\-THC
have been used. None of these methods are without difficulty and
without influence on absorption and pharmacological activity. The fact
that many methods have been used by various investigators makes
quantitative comparisons difficult.
\9\-THC is the primary active ingredient of marijuana that
produces the subjective “high” associated with smoking the plant
material and is the chemical basis for cannabis abuse. Studies in
several species of laboratory animals, including rhesus monkeys, rats
and pigeons, have found pharmacological specificity for \9\-
THC at the cannabinoid receptors, and for cannabinoid drugs that bind
with high affinity to brain cannabinoid receptors, and is psychoactive
in humans and animals (Browne and Weissman, 1981; Balster and Prescott,
1992; Compton et al., 1993; Wiley et al., 1995a,b). In general, the
doses that produce its acute therapeutic effects and its cannabimimetic
effects are similar (Devine et al., 1987; Consroe and Sandyk, 1992).
Central Nervous System Effects
It has been reported that in man, doses above 1 milligram of
\9\-THC absorbed by smoking marijuana are sufficient to cause
a “high” (Agurell et al., 1986). Further, Agurell et al. (1986)
suggested based on mouse data, that a pronounced “high” would be
caused by the presence of as little as 10 micrograms of \9\-
THC in the brain, immediately after smoking a marijuana cigarette.
These conclusions, based on a diverse array of pharmacokinetic studies,
suggest that “fiber-type” marijuana clearly has the capacity to
deposit these levels of THC into the brain of man soon after smoking a
1% THC-containing marijuana cigarette (assuming the typical “joint”
of 1 g, with 10mg THC). \9\-THC exerts its most prominent
effects on the CNS and the cardiovascular system.
Administration of \9\-THC via smoked cannabis is
associated with decrements in motivation, cognition, judgement, memory,
motor coordination, and alterations in perception (especially time
perception), sensorium, and mood (cf., Jaffe, 1993). Most commonly
\9\-THC produces an increase in well-being and euphoria
accompanied by feelings of relaxation and sleepiness. The consequences
produced by \9\-THC-induced behavioral impairments can greatly
impact the public health and safety, given that individuals may be
[[Page 20062]]
attending school, working, or driving a motor vehicle under the
influence of the drug (i.e., marijuana).
Preclinical studies show that \9\-THC produces decrements
in short-term memory, as evidenced by disruptions in acquisition and
performance of maze behavior, conditioned emotional responses, and
passive avoidance responses, impairment on the retention in delayed
matching and alternation tests, and increases in resistance to
extinction (Drew and Miller, 1974, Nakamura et al., 1991; Jaarbe and
Mathis, 1992; Lichtman and Martin, 1996). Recent studies in rats found
that these \9\-THC-induced impairments in spatial working
memory were reversible after long abstinence (Nakamura et al., 1991)
and can be blocked by the cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR141716A
(Lichtman and Martin, 1996).
Memory disturbances are one of the well-documented effects of
“\9\-THC and marijuana on human behavior (Mendelson et al.,
1974; Jaffe, 1993; Hollister, 1986; Chait and Pierri, 1992). Clinical
investigators of \9\-THC and marijuana’s effects in memory
have suggested that the drug produces a deficit in memory for recent
events, and inhibition of the passage of memory from short-term to
long-term storage (Drew and Miller, 1974; Darley 1973a,b).
Heishman, Huestis, Henningfield, & Cone (1990) demonstrated
cognitive performance decrements in marijuana smokers. Performance
remained impaired on arithmetic and recall tests on the day after smoke
administration. The authors suggested that performance decrements from
smoking two to four marijuana cigarettes may be evident for 24 to 31
hours. These data identify a particular set of performance decrements
which characterize a marijuana-induced abstinence syndrome in man.
Cardiovascular Effects
In humans, \9\-THC produces an increase in heart rate, an
increase in systolic blood pressure while supine, decreases in blood
pressure while standing, and a marked reddening of the conjunctivae
(cf., Jaffe, 1993). The increase in heart rate is dose-dependent and
its onset and duration varies but lags behind the peak of \9\-
THC levels in the blood.
Respiratory Effects
Marijuana smoking produces inflammation, edema, and cell injury in
the tracheobronchial mucosa of smokers and may be a risk factor for
lung cancer (Sarafian et al., 1999). Smoke from marijuana has been
shown to stimulate intermediate levels of reactive oxygen species. A
brief, 30-minute exposure to marijuana smoke, regardless of the THC
content, also induced necrotic cell death that increased steadily up to
48 hours after administration. Sarafian et al., concluded that
marijuana smoke containing THC is a potent source of cellular oxidative
stress that could contribute significantly to cell injury and
dysfunction in the lungs of smokers.
The low incidence of carcinogenicity may be related to the fact
that intoxication from marijuana does not require large amounts of
smoked material. This may be especially true today since marijuana has
been reported to be more potent now than a generation ago and
individuals typically titrate their drug consumption to consistent
levels of intoxication. However, several cases of lung cancer in young
marijuana users with no have been reported (Fung et al., 1999).
However, a recent study (Zhang et al., 1999, below) has suggested
that marijuana use may dose-dependently interact with mutagenic
sensitivity, cigarette smoking and alcohol use to increase the risk of
head and neck cancer. THC is known to suppress macrophage natural
killer cells and T-lymphocytes and reduce resistance to viral and
bacterial infections. As shown below, Zhu et al., demonstrated that THC
probably interacts with the T-cell cannabinoid CB2 receptor to produce
these effects. As shown in the figure, below, these researchers found
that THC promoted tumor growth in two immunocompetent mice lines. In
two different weakly immunogenic murine lung cancer models,
intermittent administration of THC led to accelerated growth of tumor
implants compared with treatment with placebo alone. The immune
inhibitory cytokines IL-10 and TGF-beta were augmented, while IFN-gamma
was down-regulated at both the tumor site and in the spleens of THC-
treated mice. This has been the first clear demonstration that THC
promotes tumor growth and supports the epidemiological evidence of an
increased risk of cancer among marijuana smokers.
In a recent comprehensive review of the existing literature base,
Carriot & Sasco (2000) reported that users under the age of 40 years of
age were more susceptible to squamous-cell carcinoma of the upper
aerodigestive tract, particularly of the tongue and larynx, and
possibly the lung. Others tumors being suspected are non-lymphoblastic
acute leukemia and astrocytoma. In head and neck cancer carcinogenicity
was observed for regular (i.e. more than once a day for years) cannabis
smokers. Moreover, cannabis increases the risk of head and neck cancer
in a dose-response manner for frequency and duration of use. THC seems
to have a specific carcinogenic effect different from that of the
pyrolysis products produced by (nicotine) cigarette smoking.
(3) The State of Current Scientific Knowledge Regarding the Drug or
Other Substance
In general, the petitioner argues that the chemistry, toxicology
and pharmacology of marijuana has been subjected to extensive study and
peer review, and have been well characterized in the scientific
literature. In addition, the discovery of the cannabinoid receptor has
shed new light on the effects of marijuana and its mechanism of action.
The literature cited by the petitioner (Tashkin et al., 1987, 1988,
1990, 1991, 1993; Barbers et al., 1991; Sherman et al., 1991a, 1991b;
Wu et al., 1992) provide data about the effects of marijuana smoke on
the lungs, which, by the petitioner’s own admission, is inherently
unhealthy. Data show that smoking marijuana is associated with more tar
than cigarettes and holding your breath (a common practice of marijuana
smokers) increases carbon monoxide concentration. His assertion that
Schedule I policy makes promoting safer marijuana smoking habits
impossible has no basis in law (exact citations are found in petition).
Pulmonary effects of smoked marijuana include bronchodilation after
acute exposure. Chronic bronchitis and pharyngitis are associated with
repeated pulmonary illness. With chronic marijuana smoking, large
airway obstruction and cellular inflammatory abnormalities appear in
bronchial epithelium (Adams and Martin, 1996). Chronic marijuana use is
associated with the same types of health problems as cigarette smoking:
increased frequency of bronchitis, emphysema and asthma. The ability of
alveolar macrophages to inactivate bacteria in the lung is impaired.
Local irritation and narrowing of airways also contribute to problems
in these patients.
Work by Perez-Reyes et al. (1991) and Agurell et al. (1989)
provides data about the pharmacokinetics of THC from smoked marijuana.
When marijuana is smoked, THC in the form of an aerosol in the
inhaled smoked is absorbed within seconds and delivered to the brain
rapidly and efficiently. Peak venous blood levels 75-150 ng/ml usually
occur by the end of smoking a cigarette and level of THC
[[Page 20063]]
in the arterial system is probably much higher (Agurell et al., 1986).
Toxicity by definition is the ability of an agent to produce injury
or cause harm (morbidity/mortality). It is not clear that the effects
of marijuana use are “well-established,” but what is known about the
psychoactive effects, lung effects, endocrine effects etc. would
suggest that smoking marijuana is not benign.
The cardiovascular effects of smoked or oral marijuana have not
presented any health problems for healthy and relatively young users.
However, marijuana smoking by older patients, particularly those with
some degree of coronary artery disease, is likely to pose greater risks
because of the resulting increased cardiac work, increased
catecholamines, carboxyhemoglobin and postural hypotension (Benzowitz
and Martin, 1996; Hollister, 1988).
The endocrine system effects include moderate depression of
spermatogenesis and sperm motility and decrease in plasma testosterone
on males. Prolactin, FSH, LH, and GH levels are decreased in females
(Mendelson and Mello, 1984). Relatively little study has been done on
human female endocrine or reproductive function.
THC and other cannabinoids in marijuana have immunosuppressant
properties producing impaired cell-mediated and humoral immune system
responses. THC and other cannabinoids suppress antibody formation,
cytokine production, leukocyte migration and killer-cell activity
(Adams and Martin, 1996).
Marijuana may cause membrane perturbations in cells. At the
marijuana conference in July, 1995 sponsored by NIH, NIDA and DHHS, Dr.
Cabral stated that THC effects body functions by accumulating in fatty
tissue. While a receptor-based mechanism of action has been determined,
localized and characterized it is not clear that this necessarily
negates membrane (high fatty acids) effects.
Mechanisms for marijuana’s psychoactive effects were thought to be
through interactions of the lipid component of cell membranes. The
discovery of the cannabinoid receptor has changed that thinking and it
is now believed that most of the effects of marijuana are mediated
through cannabinoid receptors. Receptors are located in brain areas
concerned with memory, cognition and motor coordination. An endogenous
ligand, anandamide, has been identified but not studied in humans
(Thomas et al. 1996). A specific THC antagonist, SR141716A, produces
intense withdrawal signs and behaviors in rodents that have been
exposed to THC for even a relatively short period of time (Adams and
Martin, 1996). Clinical pharmacology of the antagonist has not been
studied in humans.
Most of what is known about human pharmacology of smoked marijuana
comes from experiments with plant material containing about 2 percent
THC or less. Very few controlled studies have been done with elderly,
inexperienced or unhealthy users and data suggest that adverse effects
may differ from healthy volunteers (Hollister 1986, 1988).
Most of what is written about the pharmacological effects of
marijuana is inferred from experiments on pure THC. The amount of
Cannabidiol and other cannabinoids in smoked marijuana could modify the
effects of THC.
Tolerance to marijuana’s psychoactive effect probably results from
down regulation of cannabinoid receptors which is a form of
desensitization of neuronal cells. In general, tolerance to marijuana’s
effects is often associated with an increased dependence liability.
Data indicate that people escalate the amount of marijuana they smoke
and continue to use marijuana despite negative consequences. These are
classic signs of developing dependence.
After repeated smoked or oral marijuana doses, marked tolerance is
rapidly acquired to many of marijuana’s effects: cardiovascular,
autoimmune and many subjective effects. After exposure is stopped,
tolerance is lost with similar rapidity (Jones et al., 1981)
Withdrawal symptoms and signs appearing within hours after
cessation of repeated marijuana use have been reported in clinical
settings (Duffy and Milan, 1996; Mendelson et al., 1984). Typical
symptoms and signs were restlessness, insomnia, irritability,
salivation, diarrhea, increased body temperature and sleep disturbances
(Jones et al., 1981).
Data on the immune system indicates that marijuana does effect the
body’s ability to resist microbes including bacteria, viruses and fungi
and decreases the body’s antitumor activity. THC effects macrophages,
T-lymphocytes and B-lymphocyts. A THC receptor has been found in the
spleen. These effects may be receptor mediated. In a person with
compromised immune function marijuana could pose a health risk.
Acute effects of transient anxiety, panic, feelings of depression
and other dysphoric moods have been reported by 17 percent of regular
marijuana users in a large study (Tart, 1971). Whether marijuana can
produce lasting mood disorders or schizophrenia is less clear (IOM,
1982). Chronic marijuana use can be associated with behavior
characterized by apathy and loss of motivation along with impaired
educational performance (Pope and Yurgelun-Todd, 1996).
DEA has found that since HHS’s last medical and scientific
evaluation on marijuana (1986), there have been a significant number of
new findings relating to THC:
- Cannabinoid receptors have been identified in the brain and
spleen;
- The CNS cannabinoid receptor has been cloned;
- An endogenous arachidonic acid derivative ligand (anandamide)
has been identified;
- A high density of cannabinoid receptors have been located in the
cerebral cortex, hippocampus, striatum and cerebellum; and
- An antagonist to the cannabinoid receptor has been developed
In addition, a significant body of literature has been amassed
regarding the effects of marijuana.
For example:
- Studies on the acute and chronic effects of marijuana on the
endocrine system;
- Effect of marijuana on learning and memory;
- Effect of marijuana on pregnant females and their offspring
development;
- Effect on the immune system;
- Effect on the lungs; and
- Effects of chronic use with regard to tolerance, dependence and
“amotivational syndrome.”
While many of the petitioner’s arguments are based on new research
findings, the interpretation of those findings requires clarification.
As was pointed out by the NIH expert committee on the medical
utility of marijuana, marijuana is not a single drug. It is a variable
and complex mixture of plant parts with a varying mix of biologically
active material. Characterizing the clinical pharmacology is difficult
especially when the plant is smoked or eaten. Some of the inconsistency
or uncertainty in scientific reports describing the clinical
pharmacology of marijuana results from the inherently variable potency
of the plant material. Inadequate control over drug dose together with
the use of research subjects with variable experience in using
marijuana contributes to the uncertainty about what marijuana does or
does not do.
There are studies in the scientific literature that have evaluated
dose-related subjective and reinforcing effects of Cannabis sativa in
humans. These
[[Page 20064]]
studies have assessed the subjective and reinforcing effects of
cannabis cigarettes containing different potencies of THC and/or which
have manipulated the THC dose by varying the volume of THC smoke
inhaled (Azorlosa et al., 1992; Lukas et al., 1995; Chait et al., 1988;
Chait and Burke, 1994; Kelly et al., 1993; Kipplinger et al, 1971,
Manno et al., 1970).
Chait et al. (1988) studied the discriminative stimulus effects of
smoked marijuana cigarettes containing THC contents of 0%, 0.9%, 1.4%,
2.7%. Marijuana smokers were trained to discriminate smoked marijuana
from placebo using 4 puff of a 2.7%-THC cigarettes. Subjective ratings
of “high”, mean peak “high” scores, and physiological measures
(i.e., heart rate) were significantly and dose-dependently increased
after smoking the 0.9%, 1.4%, 2.7%. Marijuana cigarettes containing
1.4% THC completely substituted for 2.7%-THC on drug identification
tasks, however, 0.9%-THC did not. The authors found that the onset of
discriminative stimulus effects was within 90 seconds after smoking
began (after the first two puffs). Since the 1.4%-THC cigarette
substituted for 2-puffs of the 2.7%-THC cigarette, the authors estimate
that an inhaled dose of THC as low as 3 mg can produce discriminable
subjective effects.
Similarly, Lukas et al. (1995) reported that marijuana cigarettes
containing either 1.26% or 2.53% THC produced significant and dose-
dependent increases in level of intoxication and euphoria in male
occasional marijuana smokers. Four of the six subjects that smoked the
1.26%-THC cigarette reported marijuana effects and 75% of these
subjects reported euphoria. All six of the subjects that smoked 2.53%
THC reported marijuana effects and euphoria. Peak levels of self-
reported intoxication occurred at 15 and 30 minutes after smoking and
returned to control levels by 90-105 minutes. There was no difference
between latency to or duration of euphoria after smoking either the
1.26% or 2.53% THC cigarettes. The higher dose-marijuana cigarette
produced a more rapid onset and longer duration of action than the
lower dose marijuana cigarette (1.26% THC). Plasma THC levels peaked 5-
10 minutes after smoking began; the average peak level attained after
the low- and high-dose marijuana cigarette was 36 and 69 ng/ml
respectively.
In order to determine marijuana dose-effects on subjective and
performance measures over a wide dose range, Azorlosa et al. (1992)
evaluated the effects of 4, 10, or 25 puffs from marijuana cigarettes
containing 1.75 or 3.55% THC in seven male moderate users of marijuana.
Orderly dose-response curves were produced for subjective drug effects,
heart rate, and plasma concentration, as a function of THC content and
number of puffs. After smoking the 1.75% THC cigarette, maximal plasma
THC levels were 57 ng/ml immediately after smoking, 18.3 ng/ml 15
minutes after smoking, 10.3 ng/ml 30 minutes after smoking, and 7.7 ng/
ml 45 minutes after smoking.
The study also show that subjects could smoke more of the low THC
cigarette to produced effects that were similar to the high THC dose
cigarette (Azorlosa et al., 1992). There were nearly identical THC
levels produced by 10-puff low-THC cigarette (98.6 ng/ml) and 4-puff
high THC cigarette (89.4 ng/ml). Similarly, the subjective effects
ratings, including high, stoned, impaired, confused, clear-headed and
sluggish, produced under the 10 puff low- and high-THC and 25 puff low-
THC conditions did not differ significantly from each other.
As with most drugs of abuse, higher doses of marijuana are
preferred over lower dose. Although not preferred, these lower doses
still produce cannabimimetic effects. Twelve regular marijuana smokers
participated in a study designed to determine the preference of a low
potency (0.64%-THC) vs. a high potency (1.95%-THC) marijuana cigarette
(Chait and Burke, 1994). The subjects first sampled the marijuana of
two different potencies in one session, then chose which potency and
how much to smoke. During sampling sessions, there were significant
dose-dependent increases in heart rate and subjective effects,
including ratings of peak “high”, strength of drug effects,
stimulated, and drug liking. During choice sessions, the higher dose
marijuana was chosen over the lower dose marijuana on 87.5% of
occasions. Not surprising, there was a significant positive correlation
between the total number of cigarettes smoked and the ratings of
subjective effects, strength of drug effect, drug “liking”, expired
air carbon monoxide, and heart rate increases. The authors state it is
not necessary valid to assume that the preference observed in the
present study for the high-potency marijuana was due to greater CNS
effects from its higher THC content. The present study found that the
low- and high-potency marijuana cigarettes also differ on several
sensory dimensions; the high-potency THC was found to “fresher” and
“hotter”. Other studies found that marijuana cigarettes containing
different THC contents varied in sensory dimensions (cf., Chait et al.,
1988; Nemeth-Coslett et al., 1986).
As described above in Factors 1 and 2, there are data to show that
the effects of THC are dose-dependent and several studies have found
that low-potency THC is behaviorally active and can produce
cannabimimetic-like subjective and physiological effects. Preclinical
and clinical experimental data demonstrate that marijuana and
9-THC have similar abuse liabilities (i.e., drug
discrimination, self-administration, subjective effects). Both
preclinical and clinical studies show that discontinuation of either
marijuana and 9-THC administration produces a mild
withdrawal syndrome. Most of what is known about human pharmacology of
smoked marijuana comes from experiments with plant material containing
about 2-3% percent THC or less, in cigarette form provided by NIDA
(cf., NIDA, 1996). Very few controlled studies have been done with
elderly, inexperienced or unhealthy users and data suggests that
adverse effects may differ from healthy volunteers (Hollister 1986,
1988).
Cannabidiol (CBD) does not have psychotomimetic properties and does
not appear to produce a subjective “high” in human subjects (Musty,
1984). This does not mean that CBD does not have CNS effects or that it
does not contribute to the subjective high produced by the
cannabinoids. CBD has been clearly shown to have anti-convulsant
effects as demonstrated by several techniques such as electroshock-
induced seizures, kindled seizures, pentylenetetrazole-induced seizures
(Carlini et al., 1973; Izquierdo & Tannhauser, 1973). The suggestion
that CBD does not have abuse liability is based in part on the findings
that CBD does not produce THC-like discriminative stimulus effects in
animals (Ford, Balster, Dewey, Rosecrans, & Harris, 1984; but see
below). However, these tests were conducted with CBD administered alone
and at only one or two time-points (however, see Jarbe below). The
normal route of administration of THC and CBD in humans is by smoking.
This mode of administration provides a variable proportion of
cannabinoid ratios to the individual subject. As stated above, the
chemistry of marijuana is not just the chemistry of
9-THC , but at a minimum, a combination of
cannabinoids. According to Turner (1980) kinetic interactions have been
reported to occur among the cannabinoids since the early 1970s. Control
studies with varying ratios of cannabinoid administrations and
[[Page 20065]]
complete time-effect functions have still not been conducted.
Domino, Domino, & Domino (1984) have shown that the rate-of-change
of the subjective high after marijuana administration does not follow
the rate-of-change of plasma or brain THC levels. While plasma THC
function show a sharp ascending limb and exponential decline after
administration, the subjective “high” peaks after the peak in THC and
shows a protracted slow decline. The proportional ratios between the
cannabinoids and their metabolites in inhaled marijuana, acting as
entourage substances, may have emergent properties that cannot be
ascribed to any one component of the complex stimulus administered in
the smoke (Gauvin & Baird, 1999). These cannabinoid ratios may play a
critical role in the initiation, maintenance, and relapse of marijuana
smoking.
CBD has been clearly shown to have anxiolytic (Guimares et al,
1990, 1994; Musty, 1984; Onaivi, Green, & Martin, 1990; Zuardi et al.,
1982) and antipsychotic (Zuardi et al., 1995; Zuardi, Antunes
Rodrigues, & Cunha, 1991) effects in both animal and man. In the sense
that many studies which have examined the subjective profiles of
marijuana have demonstrated an “anxiety” component to THC and
marijuana use, it should not be surprising that CBD’s anxiolytic
effects block some of these discriminative properties. However, it
should not be concluded from these results that CBD’s anxiolytic
properties do not have or cannot acquire reinforcing efficacy. It has
been suggested that the affective baseline of the drug abuser plays a
critical role in the stimulus properties of drugs (Gauvin, Harland, &
Holloway, 1989). The anxiolytic properties of CBD may serve to diminish
the anxiety states associated with many psychopathological states, thus
effectively functioning as a “negative reinforcer”. As such, CBD may
function to increase the likelihood of its administration by its
ability to remove the negative affective states in anxious patients. A
number of authors have summarized the process by which marijuana
smokers “learn to get high” (cf. Jones, 1971, 1980; Cappell & Pliner,
1974). Karniol et al., (1974) have clearly demonstrated that the co-
administration of CBD with THC actually blocks the anxiety induced by
9-THC, leaving the subjects less tense and
potentiating the reinforcing effects of the THC as demonstrated by the
subjects verbal reports of enjoying the experience even more. Very few
experienced marijuana smokers report symptoms of anxiety (cf Jones,
1971, 1980; Petersen, 1980). The relief of the anxiety and/or
psychotomimetic properties of THC by the co-administration of CBD may
effectively function as a “negative reinforcer”, increasing the
likelihood of continued abuse.
Other studies have reported that cannabidiol has cannabinoid
properties, including anticonvulsant effects in animal and human models
(Consroe et al., 1981; Carlini et al., 1981; Doyle and Spence, 1995),
hypnotic effects (Monti et al., 1977), and rate-decreasing effects on
operant behavior (Hiltunen et al., 1988). Experiments with cannabidiol
in combination with THC have found that certain behavioral responses
induced by THC (i.e., operant, schedule-controlled responding) were
attenuated by cannabidiol (Borgen and Davis, 1974; Brady and Balster,
1980; Consroe et al., 1977; Dalton et al., 1976; Karniol and Carlini,
1973; Karniol et al., 1974; Welburn et al., 1976; Zuardi and Karniol,
1983; Zuardi et al., 1981, 1982; Hiltunen et al., 1988). However, other
affects produced by THC are augmented or prolonged by the combined
administration of CBD and THC or marijuana extract (Chesher and
Jackson, 1974; Hine et al., 1975a,b; Fernandes et al., 1974; Karniol
and Carlini, 1973; Musty and Sands, 1978; Zuardi and Karniol, 1983;
Zuardi et al., 1984). Still other studies did not report any behavioral
interaction between the CBD and THC (Bird et al., 1980; Browne and
Weissman, 1981; Hollister and Gillespie, 1975; Jarbe and Henricksson,
1974; Jarbe et al., 1977; Mechoulam et al., 1970; Sanders et al., 1979;
Ten Ham and DeLong, 1975).
A study to characterize the interaction between CBD and THC was
conducted using preclinical drug discrimination procedures. Rats and
pigeons trained to discriminate the presence or absence of THC, and
tested with CBD administered alone and in combinations with THC
(Hiltunen and Jarbe, 1986). Specifically, in rats trained to
discriminate 3.0 mg/kg, i.p. THC, CBD (30.0 mg/kg) was administered
alone and in combination with THC (0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg, i.p.). In pigeons
trained to discriminate 0.56 mg/kg, i.m. THC, CBD (17.5 mg/kg) was
administered alone and in combination with THC (0.1, 0.3, and 0.56 mg/
kg, i.m.). CBD prolonged the discriminative stimulus effects of THC in
rats, but did not change the time-effect curve for THC in pigeons. In
pigeons, the administration of CBD did not produce any differential
effect under a fixed ratio schedule of reinforcement (Hiltunen and
Jarbe, 1986).
These data suggest that CBD may somehow augment or prolong the
actions of THC in rats and had no effect in pigeons. In the present
study, the CBD/THC ratios ranged from 30:1 to 100:1 in rats and
enhanced the stimulus effects of THC. However, similar CBD/THC ratios
in pigeons (31:1, 58:1 and 175:1) did not result in any changes to
THC’s discriminative stimulus or response rate effects (Hiltunen and
Jarbe, 1986).
In conclusion, although cannabidiol does contribute to the other
effects of cannabis, it appears to lack cannabimimetic properties. In
addition, there does not appear to be a scientific consensus that
cannabidiol pharmacologically antagonizes, in a classic sense, the
effects of THC. Certain functional blockades have been demonstrated. As
presented in the scientific literature cited above, the ability of
cannabidiol to modify the effects of THC may be specific to only some
effects of THC. Most importantly, CBD appears to potentiate the
euphorigenic and reinforcing effects of THC which suggests that the
interaction between THC and CBD is synergistic and may actually
contribute to the abuse of marijuana.
(4) Its History and Current Pattern of Abuse
The federal databases documenting the actual abuse of marijuana are
distributed and maintained by the HHS, therefore, we acknowledge and
concur with HHS’s review of this factor analysis.
(5) The Scope, Duration, and Significance of Abuse
The basis of the petition to remove marijuana from Schedules I and
II is not based on data required by 21 U.S.C. 811 (c) (i.e., the scope,
duration, and significance of use of the substances).
The petitioner seems to assume that the concept, use of an illegal
substance is abuse of that substance, is a concept which is universally
held to the exclusion of any other definition of abuse of a substance.
While this concept is valid in general terms because marijuana is not a
legitimately marketed product therefore it has no legitimate use,
holding that all adhere to this definition of abuse denigrates the
intellectual capacity of all researchers who investigate the topic. The
petitioner neglects to recognize the efforts of the DHHS and many
groups which expend a great deal of time and money in research efforts
directed toward developing and implementing drug-abuse prevention
programs. The petitioner also rejects the notion that there are
individuals who abuse marijuana even though the National Household
Survey, to which the
[[Page 20066]]
petitioner refers, would indicate that is the case.
It has not been established that marijuana is effective in treating
any medical condition. (NIH Workshop on the Medical Utility of
Marijuana, 1997) At this time, there is no body of knowledge to which a
physician can turn to learn which medical condition in which patient
will be ameliorated at which dosage schedule of smoked marijuana nor
can he/she determine in which patient the benefits will exceed the
risks associated with such treatment. The petitioner, therefore, is
advocating that individuals become their own physicians, a notion that
even primitive man found unsatisfactory.
There is nothing absolute in the placement of a substance into a
particular CSA schedule. The placement of a substance in a CSA schedule
is the government’s mechanism for seeing that the availability of
certain psychoactive substances is limited to the industrial,
scientific and medical needs which are accepted as being legitimate.
The placement of a substance into Schedule I does not preclude research
of that substance, nor does it preclude development of a marketable
product. The National Institute on Drug Abuse, an element of the
Department of Health and Human Services, convened a conference in 1995
and with NIDA’s parent organization, the National Institutes of Health,
assembled an ad hoc group of experts in 1997 to address issues related
to the use, abuse, and medical utility of marijuana. With regard to the
medical utility of marijuana, the experts concluded that the scientific
process should be allowed to evaluate the potential therapeutic effects
of marijuana for certain disorders, dissociated from the societal
debate over the potential harmful effects of nonmedical marijuana use.
All decisions on the ultimate usefulness of a medical intervention are
based on a benefit/risk calculation, and marijuana should be no
exception to this generally accepted principle.
The cause and effect relationship which the petitioner poses is
neither substantiated nor relevant. Estimates are useful when
attempting to allocate resources but they are not necessary for
effective eradication of marijuana. Each year, millions of plants are
destroyed before their product reaches the market. In addition, federal
law enforcement activities result in the seizure of another million or
more pounds of product annually.
As reviewed by Gledhill, Lee, Strote, & Wechsler (2000), rates of
illicit drug use, especially marijuana, have risen uniformly among the
youth in the United States in the past decade and remained steady at
the end of the 1990s despite efforts to reduce prevalence. Between 1991
and 1997, rates of past 30-day marijuana use had more than doubled
among U.S. 10th grade secondary school students and more than tripled
among seniors, after a decade of decline. Between 1997 and 1999, rates
of marijuana use among secondary school students declined for the first
time in the 1990s mainly among the older students (16-17 yrs old).
Disturbing are the findings that marijuana use is steadily
increasing among 8th, 10th and 12th graders at all prevalence levels.
According to the 1996 survey results from the Monitoring the Future
Study, 45% of seniors and 35% of 10th graders claimed to have used
marijuana at least once. Among eighth graders, annual prevalence rates
more nearly tripled 1992 to 1996. Accompanying the increased use of
marijuana among High School seniors is a decreasing perceived risk or
harm of marijuana use (Johnston et al., 1996). In reality, the harm
associated with the abuse of marijuana is increasing; the marijuana
emergency room and treatment admission rates continue to increase in
recent years.
Gledhill-Hoyt, Lee, Strote, & Wechsler (2000) examined rates and
patterns of marijuana use among different types of students and
colleges in 1999, and changes in use since 1993. 15,403 students in
1993, 14,724 students in 1997, and 14,138 students in 1999 were
assessed. The prevalence of past 30-day and annual marijuana use
increased in nearly all student demographic subgroups, and at all types
of colleges. Nine out of 10 students (91%) who used marijuana in the
past 30 days had used other illicit drugs, smoked cigarettes, and/or
engaged in binge drinking. Twenty-nine percent of past 30-day marijuana
users first used marijuana and 34% began to use marijuana regularly at
or after the age of 18, when most were in college.
Coffey, Lynskey, Wolfe, & Patton (2000) examined predictors of
cannabis use initiation, continuity and progression to daily use in
adolescents. Over 2,000 students were examined. Peer cannabis use,
daily smoking, alcohol use, antisocial behavior and high rates of
school-level cannabis use were associated with middle-school cannabis
use and independently predicted high-school uptake. Cannabis use
persisted into high-school use in 80% of all middle-school users.
Middle-school use independently predicted incidents in high-school
daily use in males, while high-dose alcohol use and antisocial behavior
predicted incidence of daily use in high school females. The authors
also found that cigarette smoking was an important predictor of both
initiation and persisting cannabis use.
Farrelly et al., (2001) reviewed the NHSDA from 1990 through 1996
and compared those statistics with State law enforcement policies and
prices that affect marijuana use in the general public. These authors
found evidence that both higher fines for marijuana possession and
increased probability of arrest decreased the probability that a young
adult will use marijuana. These new data refute the petitioner’s
suggestion that legal control of marijuana does not have a dampening
effect on its use.
(6) What, if any, Risks are There to Public Health
There are human data demonstrating that marijuana and
9-THC produce an increase in heart rate, an
increase in systolic blood pressure while supine, and decreases in
blood pressure while standing (cf., Jaffe, 1993). The increase in heart
rate is dose-dependent and its onset and duration correlate with levels
of 9-THC in the blood.
When DEA evaluates a drug for control or rescheduling, the question
of whether the substance creates dangers to the public health, in
addition to, or because of, its abuse potential must be considered. A
drug substances’ risk to the public health manifests itself in many
ways. Abuse of a substance may affect the physical and/or psychological
functioning of an individual abuser. In addition, it may have
disruptive effects on the abuser’s family, friends, work environment,
and society in general. Abuse of certain substances leads to a number
of antisocial behaviors, including violent behavior, endangering
others, criminal activity, and driving while intoxicated. Data examined
under this specific factor of the CSA ranges from preclinical toxicity
to postmarketing adverse reactions in humans. DEA reviews data from
many sources, including forensic laboratory analyses, crime
laboratories, medical examiners, poison control centers, substance
abuse treatment centers, and the scientific and medical literature.
Adverse effects associated with marijuana and THC as determined by
clinical trials, FDA adverse drug effects and World Health Organization
data, are described elsewhere (cf., Chait and Zacny, 1988; Chait and
Zacny, 1992; Cone et al., 1988; and Pertwee, 1991). A recent press
release from the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Service
Administration reported that adolescents, age 12 to 17, who use
[[Page 20067]]
marijuana weekly are nine times more likely than non-users to
experiment with illegal drugs or alcohol; six times more likely to run
away from home; five times more likely to steal; nearly four times more
likely to engage in violence; and three times more likely to have
thoughts about committing suicide. It was also reported that
adolescents also associated social withdrawal, physical complaints,
anxiety, and depression, attention problems, and thoughts of suicide
with past-year marijuana use (SAMHSA, 1999). Budney, Novy, & Hughes
(1999) have recently examined the withdrawal symptomology in chronic
marijuana users seeking treatment for their dependence. The majority of
the subjects (85%) reported that they had experienced symptoms of at
least moderate severity and 47% experienced greater than four symptoms
rated as severe. The most reported mood symptoms associated with the
withdrawal state were irritability, nervousness, depression, and anger.
Some of the behavioral characteristics of the marijuana withdrawal
syndrome were craving, restlessness, sleep disruptions, strange dreams,
changes in appetite, and violent outbursts. These data clearly support
the validity and clinical significance of a marijuana withdrawal
syndrome in man.
Toxic Effects of Marijuana and THC
Although a median lethal dose (LD50) of THC has not been
established in humans, it has been found in laboratory animals
(Phillips et al., 1971). In mice, the LD50 for THC was
481.9, 454.9 and 28.6 mg/kg after oral, intraperitoneal, and
intravenous routes of administration. In rats, the LD50 for
THC (extracted from marijuana) was 666.0, 372.9 and 42.5 mg/kg after
oral, intraperitoneal, and intravenous routes of administration.
Another study examined the toxicity of THC in rats, dogs and monkeys
(Thompson et al., 1972). Similarly this study found that in rats, the
LD50 for THC was 1140.0, 400.0 and 20.0 mg/kg after oral,
intraperitoneal, and intravenous routes of administration. There was no
LD50 attained in monkeys and dogs by the oral route. Over
3000 mg/kg of THC was administered without lethality to dogs and
monkeys. A dose of about 1000 mg/kg was the lowest dose that caused
death in any animal. Behavioral changes in the survivors included
sedation, huddled postures, muscle tremors, hypersensitivity to sound
and immobility.
The cause of death in the rats and mice after oral THC was profound
depression leading to dyspnea, prostration, weight loss, loss of
righting reflex, ataxia, and severe decreases in body temperature
leading to cessation of respiration from 10 to 40 hours after a single
oral dose (Thompson et al., 1972). No consistent pathologic changes
were observed in any organs. The cause of death in dogs or monkeys
(when it rarely occurred) did not appear to be via the same mechanism
as in the rats.
In humans, the estimated lethal dose of intravenous dronabinol
[(-)-\9\-THC] is 30 mg/kg (2100 mg/70 kg). In antiemetic
studies, significant CNS symptoms were observed following oral doses of
0.4 mg/kg (28 mg/70 kg) (PDR, 1997). Signs and symptoms of mild
dronabinol intoxication include drowsiness, euphoria, heightened
sensory awareness, altered time perception, reddened conjunctiva, dry
mouth and tachycardia. Following moderate dronabinol intoxication
patients may experience memory impairment, depersonalization, mood
alterations, urinary retention, and reduced bowel motility. Signs and
symptoms of severe dronabinol intoxication include decreased motor
coordination, lethargy, slurred speech, and postural hypotension.
Dronabinol may produce panic reactions in apprehensive patients or
seizures in those with an existing seizure disorder (PDR, 1997).
Thus, large doses of THC ingested by mouth were not often
associated with toxicity in dogs, nonhuman primates and humans.
However, it did produce fatalities in rodents as a result of profound
CNS depression. Thus, the evidence from studies in laboratory animals
and human case reports indicates that the lethal dose of THC is quite
large. The adverse effects associated with THC use are generally
extensions of the CNS effects of the drug and are similar to those
reported after administration of marijuana (cf., Chait and Zacny, 1988;
Chait and Zacny, 1992; Cone et al., 1988; and Pertwee, 1991).
Health and Safety Risks of \9\-THC Use
The recent Institute of Medicine report on the scientific basis for
the medicinal use of cannabinoid products stated the following:
Not surprisingly, most users of other illicit drugs have used
marijuana first. In fact, most drug users begin with alcohol and
nicotine before marijuana–usually before they are of legal age. In
the sense that marijuana use typically precedes rather than follows
initiation of other illicit drug use, it is indeed a “gateway”
drug (Institute of Medicine Report 1999, p. ES.7).
Golub and Johnson (1994) examined the developmental pathway
followed by a sample of persons who became serious drug abusers. Of the
837 persons sampled 84% had onset to more serious drugs by the time of
the interviews. Most of the sample reported having used marijuana
(91%). Two-thirds of the drug abusers reported having used marijuana
prior to onset to more serious drugs and an additional 19% reported
having onset to marijuana and more serious drugs in the same year.
These data strongly suggest that marijuana does plan an important role
on the pathway to more serious drugs use. Further, the proportion who
onset to marijuana before or in the same year as more serious drugs was
reported to have increased substantially with time from a low of 78%
for persons born from 1928 to 1952 to 95% for the most recent birth
cohort of the study (1968-1973). These findings further suggest that
marijuana’s role as a gateway to more serious substance sue has become
more pronounced over time.
Ferguson & Horwood (2000) have examined the relationship between
cannabis use in adolescence and the onset of other illicit drug use.
Data were gathered over the course of a 21 year longitudinal study of a
birth cohort of 1,265 children. By the age of 21, just over a quarter
of this cohort reported using various forms of illicit drugs on at
least one occasion. In agreement with the predictions of a “stage-
theory” of the “gateway hypothesis” there was strong evidence of a
temporal sequence in which the use of cannabis preceded the onset of
the use of other illicit drugs. Of those reporting the use of illicit
drugs, all but three (99%) had used cannabis prior to the use of other
illicit drugs. However, the converse was not true and the majority
(63%) of those using cannabis did not progress to the use of other
forms of illicit drugs. In addition, to these findings there was a
strong dose-response relationship between the extent of cannabis use
and the onset of illicit drug use. The analysis suggested that those
using cannabis in any given year on at least 50 occasions had hazards
of using other illicit drugs that were over 140 times higher than those
who did not use in the year. Furthermore, hazards of the onset of other
illicit drug use increased steadily with increasing cannabis use. The
very strong gradient in risk reflected the facts that: (1) Among non-
users of cannabis the use of other forms of illicit drugs was almost
non-existent and (2) among regular users of cannabis the use of other
illicit drugs was common. To address the issue of “confounding
factors”, the associations between cannabis use and the onset of
illicit drug use were adjusted for a series of
[[Page 20068]]
prospectively measured confounding factors that included measures of
social disadvantage, family functioning, parental adjustment,
individual characteristics, attitudes to drug use and early adolescent
behavior. After adjustments for these factors, there was still evidence
of strong dose-response relationships between the extent of cannabis
use in a given year and the onset of illicit drug use–the hazards of
the onset of illicit drug use was 100 times those of non-users.
Critics of the “gateway theory” point to the presence of other
confounding factors and processes that encourage both cannabis use and
other forms of illicit drug use. Despite these factors, the Ferguson &
Horwood (2000) study provide a compelling set of results that support
the hypothesis that cannabis use may encourage other forms of illicit
drug use, including the following:
- Temporal sequence: There was clear evidence that the use of
cannabis almost invariably preceded the onset of other forms of
illicit drug use.
- Dose-Response: There was clear evidence of a very strong and
consistent dose-response relationship in which increasing cannabis
use was associated with increasing risks of the onset of illicit
drug use.
- Resilience to control for confounding: Even following control
for a range of prospectively measured social, family and individual
factors, strong and consistent associations remained between
cannabis use and the onset of other forms of illicit drug use. And,
- Specificity of associations: The association could not be
explained as reflecting a more general process of transition to
adolescent deviant behavior since even after control for
contemporaneously assessed measures of juvenile offending, alcohol
use, cigarette smoking, unemployment and related measures, strong
and consistent relationships between cannabis use and the onset of
other forms of illicit drugs remained.
A suggested view of the “gateway hypothesis” states that the use
of cannabis may be associated with increasing risks of other forms of
illicit drug use, with this relationship being mediated by affiliations
with deviant peers and other non-observed processes that may encourage
those who use cannabis (and particularly heavy users) to experiment
with, and use, other illicit drugs.
While marijuana is clearly not the only gateway to the use of other
illicit drugs it is one of the three most typical drugs in the
adolescent’s armamentarium. The increased avenues to imported and
“home-grown” marijuana which contain behaviorally-active doses of THC
and CBD pose a serious threat to the health and well-being of this
dimension of society.
Taylor et al. (2000) evaluated the relationship between cannabis
dependence and respiratory symptoms and lung function in young adults,
21 years of age, while controlling for the effects of cigarette
smoking. The researchers found significant respiratory symptoms and
changes in spirometry occur in cannabis-dependent individuals at age 21
years, even though the cannabis smoking history is of relatively short
duration. The likelihood of reporting a broad range of respiratory
symptoms was significantly increased in those who were either cannabis-
dependent or smoked tobacco or both compared to non-smokers. The
symptoms most frequently and significantly associated with cannabis
dependence were early morning sputum production (144% greater
prevalence than non-smokers). Overall, respiratory symptoms in study
members who met strict criteria for cannabis dependence were comparable
to those of tobacco smokers consuming 1-10 cigarettes daily. In
subjects who were both tobacco users and were cannabis-dependent, some
effects seem to be additive, notably early morning sputum production,
which occurred 8 times more frequently than non-smokers.
One of the greatest concerns to society regarding \9\-THC
is the behavioral toxicity produced by the drug. \9\-THC
intoxication is associated with impairments in memory, motor
coordination, cognition, judgement, motivation, sensation, perception
and mood (cf., Jaffe, 1993). The consequences produced by \9\-
THC-induced behavioral impairments can greatly impact the individual
and society in general. These impairments result in occupational,
household, or airplane, train, truck, bus or automobile accidents,
given that individuals may be attending school, working, or operating a
motor vehicle under the influence of the drug. In the most general
sense, impaired driving can be seen as a failure to exercise the
expected degree of prudence or control necessary to ensure road safety.
The operations of a motor vehicle are clearly a skilled performance
that requires controlled and flexible use of a person’s intellectual
and perceptual resources. Cannabis interferes with resource allocations
in both cognitive and attentional tasks.
In 1999, Ehrenreich et al., examined the detrimental effects of
chronic interference by cannabis with the endogenous cannabinoid
systems during peripubertal development in humans. As an index of
cannabinoid action, visual scanning and other attentional factors were
examined in 99 individuals who exclusively used cannabis. Early-onset
cannabis use (onset before the age of 16) showed significant
impairments in attention in adulthood. These persistent attentional
deficits may interact with the activities of daily living, such as
operating an automobile.
Kurzthaler et al., (1999) examined the effects of cannabis on a
cognitive test battery and driving performance skills. The demonstrated
significant impairments in the verbal memory and the trail making tests
in this study reflect parallel compromises in associative control that
is acknowledged as a cognitive process inherent in memory function
immediately after smoking cannabis. Applied to the question of driving
ability, the authors suggest that the missing functions would signify
that a driver under acute cannabis influences would not be able to use
acquired knowledge from earlier experiences adequately to ensure road
safety.
Recently, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration
(NHTSA; 1998, 1999, 2000) conducted a study with the Institute for
Human Psychopharmacology at Maastricht University in The Netherlands.
Low dose and high dose THC administered alone, and with alcohol were
examined in two on-road driving situations: (1) The Road Tracking Test,
measuring a driver’s ability to maintain a constant speed of 62 mph and
a steady lateral position between the boundaries of the right traffic
lane; and (2) the Car Following Test, measuring a drivers’ reaction
times and ability to maintain distance between vehicles while driving
164 ft. behind a vehicle that executed a series of alternating
accelerations and decelerations. Both levels of THC alone, and alcohol
alone, significantly impaired performances on BOTH road tests compared
with baseline. Alcohol and the high dose of THC produced 36% decrements
in reaction time; because the test vehicles were traveling at 59 mph,
the delayed reaction times meant that the vehicle traveled, on average,
an additional 139 feet beyond the point where the subjects began to
decelerate. Even the lower dose of THC by itself retarded reaction
times by 0.9 seconds. The NHTSA concluded that even in low to moderate
doses, marijuana impairs driving performance.
In a related analysis, Yesavage, Leirer, Denari, & Hollister (1985)
examined the acute and delayed effects of smoking one marijuana
cigarette containing 1.9% THC (19 mg of THC) on aircraft pilot
performance. Ten private pilot licensed subjects were trained in a
flight simulator prior to marijuana exposure. Flight simulator
performance was
[[Page 20069]]
measured by the number of aileron (lateral control), elevator (vertical
control) and throttle changes; the size of these control changes; the
distance off the center of the runway on landing; and the average
lateral and vertical deviation from an ideal glideslope and center line
over the final mile of the approach. Compared to baseline performance,
significant differences occurred in all variables at 1 and 4 hours
after smoking, except for the numbers of throttle and elevator changes
at 4 hours. Most importantly, at 24 hours after a single marijuana
cigarette, there were significant impairments in the number and size of
aileron (lateral control) changes, size of elevator changes, distance
off-center on landing, and vertical and lateral deviations on approach
to landing. Interestingly, despite these performance deficits, the
pilots reported no significant subjective awareness of their
impairments at 24 hours. It is noteworthy that a fatal crash in which a
pilot had a positive THC screen involved similar landing misjudgments.
In addition to causing unsafe conditions, marijuana use results in
decreased performance and lost productivity in the workplace, including
injuries, absenteeism, and increased health care costs. A NIDA report
on drugs in the workplace summarized the prevalence of marijuana use in
the workplace and its impact on society. This report found that in
1989, one in nine working people (11%) reported current use of
marijuana (Gust and Walsh, 1989). Recent DAWN data and other surveys
indicate that marijuana use is increasing, especially among younger and
working age individuals.
Bray, Zarkin, Ringwalt, & Qi (2000) estimated the impact of age of
dropout on the relationship between marijuana use and high school
dropouts using four longitudinal surveys from students in the
Southeastern U.S. public school system. Their results suggested that
marijuana initiation was positively related to high school dropout.
Although the magnitude and the significance of the relationship varied
with age of dropout and the other substances used, the overall effect
represented an odds-ratio of approximately 2.3. These data suggest that
an individual is approximately 2.3 times more likely to drop out of
school than an individual who has not initiated marijuana use.
When DEA evaluates a drug for control or rescheduling, whether the
substance creates dangers to the public health, in addition to or
because of its abuse potential, must be considered. The risk to the
public health of a substance may manifest itself in many ways. Abuse of
a substance may affect the physical and/or psychological functioning of
an individual abuser, it may have disruptive effects on the abuser’s
family, friends, work environment, and society in general. Abuse of
certain substances leads to a number of antisocial behaviors, including
violent behavior, endangering others, criminal activity, and driving
while intoxicated. Data examined under this factor ranges from
preclinical toxicity to postmarketing adverse reactions in humans. DEA
reviews data from many sources, including forensic laboratory analyses,
crime laboratories, medical examiners, poison control centers,
substance abuse treatment centers, and the scientific and medical
literature.
In its official report titled “Marijuana and Medicine: Assessing
the Science Base”, the Institute of Medicine highlighted a number of
risks to the public health as a result of cannabis consumption:
(1) Cognitive impairments associated with acutely administered
marijuana limit the activities that people would be able to do
safely or productively. For example, no one under the influence of
marijuana or THC should drive a vehicle or operate potentially
dangerous equipment (Page 107).
(2) The most compelling concerns regarding marijuana smoking in
HIV/AIDS patients are the possible effects of marijuana on immunity.
Reports of opportunistic fungal and bacterial pneumonia in AIDS
patients who used marijuana suggest that marijuana smoking either
suppresses the immune system or exposes patients to an added burden
of pathogens. In summary, patients with pre-existing immune deficits
due to AIDS should be expected to be vulnerable to serious harm
caused by smoking marijuana. The relative contribution of marijuana
smoke versus THC or other cannabinoids is not known. (Page 116-117)
(3) DNA alterations are known to be early events in the
development of cancer, and have been observed in the lymphocytes of
pregnant marijuana smokers and in those of their newborns. This is
an important study because the investigators were careful to exclude
tobacco smokers; a problem in previous studies that cited mutagenic
effects of marijuana smoke. (Page 118-119)
(4) * * * factors influence the safety of marijuana or
cannabinoid drugs for medical use: the delivery system, the use of
plant material, and the side effects of cannabinoid drugs. (1)
Smoking marijuana is clearly harmful, especially in people with
chronic conditions, and is not an ideal drug delivery system. (2)
Plants are of uncertain composition, which renders their effects
equally uncertain, so they constitute an undesirable medication.
(Page 127)
(7) Its Psychic or Physiological Dependence Liability
The “dopaminergic hypothesis of drug abuse” is not the only
explanation for the neurochemical actions of drugs. The nucleus
accumbens/ventral striatum areas of the brain, typically referred to as
simply the Nucleus Accumbens (NAc), represents a critical site for
mediating the rewarding or hedonic properties of several classes of
abused drugs, including alcohol, opioids, and psychomotor stimulants
(Gardner & Vorel, 1998; Koob, 1992; Koob et al., 1998; Wise, 1996; Wise
& Bozarth, 1987). It is generally appreciated that all of these drugs
augment extracellular dopamine levels in the NAc and that this action
contributes to their rewarding properties. However, recent evidence
also suggests that many drugs of abuse have dopamine-independent
interactions with Nac neuronal activity (Carlezon & Wise, 1996; Chieng
& Williams, 1998; Koob, 1992; Martin et al., 1997; Yuan et al., 1992).
Recent studies conducted at the Cellular Neurobiology Branch of the
NIDA by Hoffman & Lupica (2001) concluded that THC modulates NAc
glutamatergic functioning of dopamine. These authors suggested that
increases in Nac dopamine levels may be a useful neurochemical index of
drug reward but do not fully account for the complex processing of fast
synaptic activity by this neuromodulator in the Nac. Moreover, because
both glutamatergic and GABAergic inputs to medium spiny neurons are
directly inhibited by dopamine, as well as by drugs of abuse. It is
likely that these effects contribute to the abuse liability of
marijuana.
In addition, the petitioner’s global statements about the role of
dopamine, the reinforcing effects of marijuana and other drugs, and the
predictive validity of animal self-administration studies with
marijuana and abuse potential in humans are not supported by the
scientific literature. For example:
(1) There are drugs that do not function through dopaminergic
systems that are self-administered by animals and humans (i.e.,
barbiturates, benzodiazepines, PCP).
(2) There are drugs that are readily self-administered by animals
that are not abused by man (antihistamines)
(3) There are drugs that are abused by humans that are not readily
self-administered by animals (hallucinogens and hallucinogenic
phenethylamines, nicotine, caffeine).
(4) There are drugs that have no effect on dopamine that are self-
administered
[[Page 20070]]
by animals and not abused by humans (i.e., antihistamines).
Physical Dependence in Animals
Abrupt withdrawal from 9-THC can produce a mild
spontaneous withdrawal syndrome in animals, including increased motor
activity and grooming in rats, decreased seizure threshold in mice and
increased aggressiveness, irritability and altered operant performance
in rhesus monkeys (cf., Pertwee, 1991). The failure to observe profound
withdrawal signs following abrupt discontinuation of
9-THC may be due to (1) its long half-life in
plasma and (2) slowly waning levels of 9-THC and
its metabolites that continue to permit receptor adaptation.
Recently the discovery of a cannabinoid receptor antagonist
demonstrates that a profound precipitated withdrawal syndrome can be
produced in 9-THC tolerant animals after twice
daily injections (Tsou et al., 1995) or continuous infusion (Aceto et
al., 1995, 1996). In rats continuously infused with low doses
9-THC for four days, the cannabinoid antagonist
precipitated a behavioral withdrawal syndrome, including scratching,
face rubbing, licking, wet dog shakes, arched back and ptosis (Aceto et
al., 1996). This chronic low dose regimen consisted of 0.5, 1, 2, 4 mg/
kg/day 9-THC on days 1 through 4; 5 and 25-fold
higher 9-THC doses were used for the medium and
high dose regimens, respectively. The precipitated withdrawal syndrome
was dose-dependently more severe in the medium and high THC dose
groups.
Physical Dependence in Humans
Signs of withdrawal have been demonstrated after studies with
9-THC. Although the intensity of the withdrawal
syndrome is related to the daily dose and frequency of administration,
in general, the signs of 9-THC withdrawal have been
relatively mild (cf., Pertwee, 1991). This withdrawal syndrome has been
compared to that of a short-term, low dose treatment with an opioid or
ethanol, and includes changes in mood, sleep, heart rate body
temperature, and appetite. Other signs such as irritability,
restlessness, tremor mild nausea, hot flashes and sweating have also
been noted (cf., Jones, 1983).
A withdrawal syndrome was reported after the discontinuation of
oral THC in volunteers receiving dronabinol dosages of 210 mg/day for
12 to 16 consecutive days (PDR, 1997). This was 42-times the
recommended dose of 2.5 mg, b.i.d. Within 12 hours after
discontinuation, these volunteers manifested withdrawal symptoms such
as irritability, insomnia, and restlessness. By approximately 24 hours
after THC discontinuation, there was an intensification of withdrawal
symptoms to include “hot flashes”, sweating, rhinorrhea, loose
stools, hiccoughs, and anorexia. These withdrawal symptoms gradually
dissipated over the next 48 hours. EEG changes consistent with the
effects of drug withdrawal (hyperexcitation) were recorded in patients
after abrupt challenge. Patients also complained of disturbed sleep for
several weeks after discontinuation of high doses of dronabinol. The
intensity of the cannabinoid withdrawal syndrome is related by the
chronic dose and by the frequency of chronic administration. There is
also evidence that the cannabinoid withdrawal symptoms can be reversed
by the administration of marijuana and 9-THC, or by
treatment with a barbiturate (hexobarbital) or ethanol (Pertwee, 1991).
An acute withdrawal syndrome or “hangover” has been reported by
Chait, Fischman, & Schuster (1985) developing approximately 9 hours
after smoking a 1 g marijuana cigarette containing 2.9% THC. Five of
twelve subjects reported themselves as “dopey and hung over” the
morning after smoking the single cigarette. In a 10 second and 30
second time-production task significant marijuana hangover effects were
found. The effect on the time production task is of interest since the
effect obtained the morning after smoking marijuana was opposite to
that observed acutely after smoking marijuana. These data may suggest
an opponent compensatory rebound which may underlie the development of
tolerance over periods of chronic marijuana exposure. Scores on the
benzedrine-group (BG) scale, a stimulant scale of the Addiction
Research Center Inventory (ARCI) consisting mainly of terms relating to
intellectual efficiency and energy, were significantly higher the
morning after marijuana smoking, as well. Chait, Fischman, & Schuster
also reported increases on the amphetamine (A) scale of the ARCI, a
measure of the dose-related effects of d-amphetamine. Cousens &
DiMascio (1973) have previously reported a similar “hangover” and
“speed of thought alterations” in subjects the morning after they had
received a 30 mg oral dose of 9-THC. Like the
“hangover” associated with high dose ethyl alcohol consumption, the
hangover from marijuana may be qualitatively identical to, and differ
only on an intensity dimension from, the withdrawal syndrome produced
from chronic consumption (cf. Gauvin, Cheng, Holloway, 1993).
As described above, Haney et al. have recently described abstinence
symptoms of an acute withdrawal syndrome following high (30 mg q.i.d.)
and low (20 mg q.i.d) dose administrations of oral THC (Haney et al.,
1999a) and following 5 puffs of high (3.1%) and low (1.8%) THC-
containing smoked marijuana cigarettes (Haney et al., 1999b). Both of
these studies have delineated a withdrawal syndrome from concentrations
of THC significantly lower than those reported in any other previous
study and, for the first time, clearly identified a marijuana
withdrawal syndrome detected at low levels of THC exposure that do not
produce tolerance. These data suggest that dependence on THC may in
fact be an important consequence of repeated, daily exposure to
cannabinoids and that daily marijuana use may be maintained, at least
in part, by the alleviation of abstinence symptoms.
As stated above, Budney, Novy, & Hughes (1999) have recently
examined the withdrawal symptomology in chronic marijuana users seeking
treatment for their dependence. The majority of the subjects (85%)
reported that they had experienced symptoms of at least moderate
severity and 47% experienced greater than four symptoms rated as
severe. The most reported mood symptoms associated with the withdrawal
state were irritability, nervousness, depression, and anger. Some of
the behavioral characteristics of the marijuana withdrawal syndrome
were craving, restlessness, sleep disruptions, strange dreams, changes
in appetite, and violent outbursts. These data clearly support the
validity and clinical significance of a marijuana withdrawal syndrome
in man. Large-scale population studies have also reported significant
rates of cannabis dependence (Kessler et al., 1994; Farrell et al.,
1998), particularly in prison and homeless populations. Similar reports
of cannabis dependence in withdrawal in other populations have been
previously discussed (above; Crowley et al. (1998); Kouri & Pope
(2000)).
Psychological Dependence in Humans
In addition to the physical dependence produced by abrupt
withdrawal from 9-THC, psychological dependence on
9-THC can also be demonstrated. Case reports and
clinical studies show that frequency of 9-THC use
(most often as marijuana) escalates over time, there is evidence that
individuals increase the number, doses, and potency of marijuana
cigarettes. Data have clearly shown that tolerance
[[Page 20071]]
to the stimulus effects of the drug develops which could lead to drug
seeking behavior (Pertwee, 1991; Aceto et al., 1996; Kelly et al.,
1993, 1994; Balster and Prescott, 1992; Mendelson et al., 1976;
Mendelson and Mello, 1985; Mello, 1989). Several studies have reported
that patterns of marijuana smoking and increased quantity of marijuana
smoked were related to social context and drug availability (Kelly et
al., 1994; Mendelson and Mello, 1985; Mello, 1989). There have been,
however, other studies which have demonstrated that the magnitude of
many of the behavioral effects produced by 9-THC
and other synthetic cannabinoids lessens with repeated exposure while
also demonstrating that tolerance did not develop to the euphorigenic
activity, or the “high” from smoked marijuana (Dewey, 1986; Perez-
Reyes et al., 1991). Recent electrophysiological data from animals
suggests that the response of VTA dopamine neurons do not diminish
during repeated exposure to cannabinoids, and that this may underlie
the lack of tolerance to the euphoric effects of marijuana even with
chronic use (Wu & French, 2000).
The problems of psychological dependence associated with marijuana
(THC) abuse are apparent from DAWN reports and survey data from the
National Household Survey on Drug Abuse and the Monitoring the Future
study. These databases show that the incidence of chronic daily
marijuana use and adverse events associated with its use are
increasing, especially among the young. At the same time, perception of
risk has decreased and availability is widespread (cf., NIDA, 1996).
These factors contribute to perpetuating the continued use of the
marijuana.
(8) Whether The Substance Is an Immediate Precursor of a Substance
Already Controlled Under This Subchapter.
According to the legal definition, marijuana (Cannabis sativa L.)
is not an immediate precursor of a scheduled controlled substance.
However, cannabidiol is a precursor for delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, a
Schedule I substance under the CSA.
References
Aceto MD, Scates SM, Lowe JA, & Martin BR (1995). Cannabinoid
precipitated withdrawal by the selective cannabinoid receptor
antagonist, SR 141716A. Eur J Pharmacol 282:R1-R2.
Aceto MD, Scates SM, Lowe JA, & Martin BR (1996). Dependence on
9-tetrahydrocannabinol: studies on precipitated
and abrupt withdrawal. J Pharmacol Exper Therap 278:1290-1295.
Adams IB & Martin BR (1996). Cannabis: Pharmacology and toxicology
in animals and humans. Addiction 91:1585-1614.
Agurell S, Gillespie H, Halldin M, Hollister LE, Johansson E,
Lindgren JE, Ohlsson A, Szirmai M, & Widman M (1984). A review of
recent studies on the pharmacokinetics and metabolism of delta-1-
tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabidiol and cannabinol in man. In: Harvey
DJ (Ed), Marijuana ’84. Proceedings of the Oxford Symposium on
Cannabis. IRL Press Ltd:Oxford, England, pp. 49-62.
Agurell S, Halldin M, Lindgren J E et al. (1986). Pharmacokinetics
and metabolism of delta-1-tetrahydrocannabinoid and other
cannabinoids with emphasis on man. Pharmacol Rev 38:21-43.
Agurell S, Leander K (1971). Stability, transfer and absorption of
cannabinoid constituents of Cannabis (Hashish) during smoking. Acta
Pharm Succica 8:391-402.
Azorlosa J, Heishman S, Stitzer M (1992). Marijuana smoking: effect
of varying delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol content and number of puffs.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther 261:114-122.
Baker PB, Gough TA, Taylor BJ (1982). The physical and chemical
features of Cannabis plants grown in the United Kingdom of Great
Britain and Northern Ireland from seeds of known origin. Bull Narc
34:27-36.
Baker PB, Gough TA, Taylor BJ (1983). The physical and chemical
features of Cannabis plants grown in the United Kingdom of Great
Britain and Northern Ireland from seeds of know origin–Part II:
second generation studies. Bull Narc 35:51-62.
Balster RL & Prescott WR (1992). 9-tetrahydrocannabinol
discrimination in rats as a model for cannabis intoxication.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 16:55-62.
Barnett G, Chiang C-WN, Perez-Reyes M, Owens SM (1982). Kinetic
study of smoking marijuana. J Pharmacokin Biopharm 10:495-505.
Barrett RL, Wiley JL, Balster RL & Martin BR (1995). Pharmacological
specificity of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol discrimination in
rats. Psychopharmacology 118:419-424.
Beal JA, & Martin BM (1995). The clinical management of wasting and
malnutrition in HIV/AIDS. AIDS Patient Care April: 66-74.
Becker HS (1967). History, culture and subjective experience: an
exploration of the social bases of drug-induced experiences. J
Health Soc Behav 8:163-176.
Benowitz NL, & Jones RT (1981). Cardiovascular and metobolic
considerations in prolonged cannabinoid administration in man. J
Clin Pharmacol 21:214S-223S.
Bird KD, Boleyn T, Chesher GB, Jackson DM, Starmer GA, & Teo RKC
(1980). Intercannabinoid and cannabidiol-ethanol interactions and
their effects on human performance. Psychopharmacology 71:181-188.
Bornheim LM, Kim KY, Li J, Perotti BYT, Benet LZ (1995). Effect of
cannabidiol pretreatment on the kinetics of tetrahydrocannabinol
metabolites in mouse brain. Drug Metab Dispos 23:825-831.
Borgen LA, & Davis WM (1974). CBD interaction with 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol. Res Commun Chem Pathol Pharmacol 7:663-670.
Bouquet RJ (1951). Cannabis. Bull Narc 3:14-30.
Brady JV, Hienz RD, & Ator NA (1990). Stimulus functions of drugs
and the assessment of abuse liability. Drug Develop Res 20:231-249.
Brady KT, & Balster RL (1980) the effects of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol alone and in combination with cannabidiol on
fixed-interval performance in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology
72:21-26.
Braut-Boucher F, Paris M, Cosson L (1977). Mise en evidence de deux
types chimiques chez le Cannabis sativa originaire d’Afrique du sud.
Phytochemistry 16:1445-1448.
Braut-Boucher F (1978). Etude ecophysiologique et chimique due
cannabis sativa L. cultive en Phytotron. Mise en evidence d’un type
chimique nouveau chez un Chanvre originaire d’Afrique due Sud.
Doctoral thesis. University of Paris XI.
Bray JW, Zarkin GA, Ringwalt C, Qi J (2000). The relationship
between marijuana initiation and dropping out of high school. Health
Econ 9:9-18.
Braut-Boucher F, & Petiard V (1981). Sur la mise en culture in vitro
de tissue de differents types chimiques de Cannabis sativa L. C R
Acad Sci (Paris) 292:833-838.
Browne RG, & Weissman A (1981). Discriminative stimulus properties
of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol: Mechanistic studies. J Clin
Pharmacol 21:227S-234S.
Budney AJ, Novy PL, Hughes JR (1999). Marijuana withdrawal among
adults seeking treatment for marijuana dependence. Addiction
94:1311-1321.
Caldwell DF, Myers SA, Domino EF, & Merriam PE (1969a). Auditory and
visual threshold effects of marihuana in man. Percept Motor Skills
29:755-759.
Caldwell DF, Myers SA, Domino EF, & Merriam PE (1969b). Auditory and
visual threshold effects of marihuana in man:Addendum. Percept Motor
Skills 29:922.
Cappell H, & Pliner P (1974). Cannabis intoxication: the role of
pharmacological and psychological variables. In: Miller LL (Ed),
Marijuana: Effects on human behavior. Academic Press:New York, pp.
233-264.
Carlezon WAJ, Wise RA (1996). Rewarding actions of phencyclidine and
related drugs in nucleus accumbens shell and frontal cortex. J
Neurosci 16:3112-3122.
Carlini EA, & Cunha JA (1981). Hypnotic and antiepileptic effects of
cannabidiol. J Clin Pharmacol 32:417-427.
Carlini EA, Karniol IG, Renault PF, Schuster CR (1974). Effects of
marijuana in laboratory animals and in man. Br J Pharmacol 50:299-
309.
Carlini EA, Leite JR, Tannhauser M, Berardi
[[Page 20072]]
AC (1973). Letter: Cannabidiol and cannabis sativa extract protect
mice and rats against convulsive agents. J Pharm Pharmacol 25:664-
665.
Carney JM, Uwaydah IM, & Balster RL (1977). Evaluation of a
suspension system for intravenous self-administration studies of
water-insoluble compounds in the rhesus monkey. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav 7:357-364.
Carriot F, Sasco AJ (2000). Cannabis and cancer. Rev Epidemiol Sante
Publique 48:473-483.
Chait LD, Burke KA (1994). Preference for “high” versus low-
potency marijuana. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 49:643-647.
Chait LD, Fischman MW & Schuster CR (1985). “Hangover” effects the
morning after marijuana smoking. Drug Alcohol Depend 15:229-238.
Chait LD, & Zacny JP (1992). Reinforcing and subjective effects of
oral \9\-THC and smoked marijuana in humans.
Psychopharmacology 107:255-262.
Chait LD, & Pierri J (1992). Effects of smoked marijuana on human
performance. In: Murphy L, & Bartke A (Eds). Marijuana/Cannabinoids.
Neurobiology and Neurophysiology. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL; pp.
387-423.
Chait LD, Evans SM, Grant KA, Kamien JB, Johanson CE, & Schuster CR
(1988). Discriminative stimulus and subjective effects of smoked
marijuana in humans. Psychopharmacology 94:206-212.
Chen J, Paredes W, Li J, Smith D, Lowinson J, & Gardner EI (1994).
Psychopharmacology 102:156-162.
Chesher GB, & Jackson DM (1974). Anticonvulsant effets of
cannabinoids in mice: drug interactions within cannabinoids and
cannabinoid interactions with phenytoin. Psychopharmacologia (Berl)
37:255-264.
Chieng B, Williams JT (1998). Increase opioid inhibition of GABA
release in nucleus accumbens during morphine withdrawal. J Neurosci
18:7033-7039.
Clark LD, & Nakashima EC (1968). Experimental studies with
marihuana. Am J Psychiat 125:135-140.
Cocchetto DM, Owens SM, Perez-Reyes M, DiGuiseppi S, Miller LL
(1981). Relationship between delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol
concentration and pharmacologic effects in man. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 75:158-164.
Coffey C, Lynskey M, Wolfe R, Patton GC (2000). Initiation and
progression of cannabis use in a population-based Australian
adolescent longitudinal study. Addiction 95:1679-1690.
Community Epidemiology Work Group. (1995). Epidemiological trends in
Drug Abuse, December 1994: Volume 1: Highlights and Executive
Summary. National Institute on Drug Abuse, NIH Publication No. 95-
3988, pp. 54-56.
Compton DR, Rice KC, DeCosta BR, Razdan RK, Melvin LS, Johnson MR, &
Martin BR (1993). Cannabinoid structure-activity relationships:
correlation of receptor binding and in vivo activities. J Pharmacol
Exper Ther 265:218-226.
Cone EJ, Johnson RE, Paul BD, Mell LD, & Mitchell J (1988).
Marijuana-laced brownies: Behavioral effects, physiological effects
and urinalysis in humans following ingestion. J Anal Toxicol 12:169-
175.
Consroe P, Martin P, & Eisenstein D (1977). Anticonvulsant drug
antagonism of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol seizures in
rabbits. Res Commun Chem Pathol Pharmacol 16:1-13.
Consroe P, Martin P, & Singh V (1981). Antiepileptic potential of
cannabidiol analogues. J Clin Pharmacol 21:428S-436S.
Cousens K, DiMascio A (1973). (-)9 THC as an
hypnotic: An experimental study of three dose levels.
Psychopharmacologia (Berl.) 33:355-364.
Crancer JM, Dille JM, Delay JC, Wallace JE, Haykin MD (1969).
Comparisons of the effects of marihuana and alcohol on simulated
driving performance. Science 164:851-854.
Crowley TJ, Macdonald MJ, Whitmore EA, Mikulich SK (1998). Cannabis
dependence, withdrawal, and reinforcing effects among adolescents
with conduct symptoms and substance use disorders. Drug Alcohol
Depend 50:27-37.
Dalton WS, Martz R, Kenberger L, Rodda BE, & Forney RB (1976).
Influence of cannabidiol on delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects.
Clin Pharmacol Therap 19:300-309.
Darley CF, Tinklenbreg WT, Roth WT, Hollister LE, & Atkinson RC
(1973a). Influence of marihuana on storage and retrieval processes
in memory. Mem Cognit 1:196-200.
Darley CF, Tinklenbreg WT, Hollister LE, & Atkinson RC (1973b).
Marihuana and retrieval from short term memory. Psychopharmacologia
(Berl.) 29:231-233.
deMeijer EPM (1993). Hemp variations as pulp source researched in
the Netherlands. Government-funded hemp (Cannabis sativa L.)
investigation evaluates stem quality, yield, comparison to
woodfibers. Pulp & Paper 67:41-43.
deMeijer EPM, van der Kamp HJ, & van Eeuwijk VA (1992).
Characterisation of cannabis accessions with regard to cannabinoid
content in relation to other plant characters. Euphytica 62:187-200.
Deneau GA, & Kaymakcalan S. (1971). Physiological and psychological
dependence to 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in
rhesus monkeys. Pharmacologist 13:246.
Devine ML, Dow GJ, Greenberg BR, Holsten DW, Icaza L, Jue PY, Meyers
FH, O’Brien E, Roberts CM, Rocchio GL, Stanton W, & Wesson DL
(1987). Adverse reactions to 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol given as an antiemetic in a multicenter study.
Clin Pharmacol 6:319-322.
Dewey WL (1986). Cannabinoid pharmacology. Pharmacol Rev 38: 151-
178.
deWit H, & Griffiths RR (1991). Testing the abuse liability of
anxiolytic and hypnotic drugs in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend 28:83-
111.
deWit H, Bodker B, Ambre J (1992). Rate of increase of plasma drug
level influences subjective response in humans. Psychopharmacology
107:352-358.
DiMarzo V, Melis M, Gessa GL (1998). Endocannabinoids: endogenous
cannabinoid receptor ligands with neuromodulatory actions. TINS
21:521-528.
Domino LE, Domino SE, Domino EF (1984). Relation of plasma delta-9-
THC concentrations to subjective “high” in marijuana users: A
review and reanalysis. In: S Agurell, WL Dewey, Willette RE (Eds)
The Cannabinoids: chemical, pharmacologic, and therapeutic aspects.
Orlando, FL: Academic Press, pp 245-261.
Domino EF, Rennick P, & Pearl JH (1976). Short-term
neuropsychopharmacological effects of marihuana smoking in
experienced male users. In: Braude MC & Szara S (Eds) The
Pharmacology of Marihuana. Raven Press: New York, pp 393-412.
Doorenbos NJ, Fetterman PS, Quimby MW, Turner CE (1971). Cultivation
extraction and analysis of Cannabis sativa L.. Ann NY Acad Sci
191:3-15.
Doyle E, & Spence AA (1995). Cannabis as a medicine. Br J Anaesth
74:359-361.
Drew G, & Miller L (1974). Cannabis: Neural mechanisms and
behavior–a theoretical review. Pharmacol Rev 38:151-178.
Duffy A, & Milin R (1996). Withdrawal syndrome in adolescent chronic
cannabis users. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35:1618-1621.
Ehrenreich H, Rinn T, Kunert HJ, Moeller MR, Poser W, Schilling L,
Gigerenzer G, Hoehe MR (1999). Specific attentional dysfunction in
adults following early start of cannabis use. Pyschopharmacology
(Berl) 142:295-301.
Fairbairn JW, Hindmarch I, Simic S, Tylden E (1974). Cannabinoid
content of some English reefers. Nature 249: 276-278.
Fairbairn JW, & Liebmann JA (1974). The cannabinoid content of
cannabis sativa L.. grown in England. J Pharmac Pharmacol 26:413-
419.
Fairbairn JW, Liebmann JA, & Simic S (1971). The
tetrahydrocannabinol content of cannabis leaf. J Pharmac Pharmacol
23:558-559.
Farre M, & Cami J (1991) Pharmacokinetics and abuse liability. Br J
Addict 86:1601-1606.
Farrell M, Howes S, Taylor C, Lewis G, Jenkins R, Bebbington P,
Jarvis M, Brugha T, Gill B, Meltzer H (1998). Substance misuse and
psychiatric comorbidity: an overview of the OPCS national
psychiatric morbidity survey. Addictive Behaviors 23:909-918.
Farrelly MC, Bray JW, Zarkin GA, Wendling BW (2001). The joint
demand for cigarettes and marijuana: evidence from the National
Household Surveys on Drug Abuse. J Health Econ 20:51-68.
Fernandes M, Schabarak A, Coper H, & Hill R (1974). Modificarion of
the 9-THC-actions by cannabinol and cannabidiol
in the rats. Psychopharmacologia (Berl.) 38:329-338.
Fetterman PS, Doorenbos NJ, Keith ES, Quimby MW (1971). A simple gas
liquid chromatography procedure for determination of cannabinoidic
acids in
[[Page 20073]]
Cannabis sativa L. Experientia 27:988-989.
Fetterman PS, Keith ES, Waller CW, Guerrero O, Doorenbos NJ, Quimby
MW (1970). Mississippi grown Cannabis sativa L.. A preliminary
observation on the chemical definition of phenotype and variations
in the content versus age, sex, and plant park. J Pharm Sci 60:1246-
1249.
Ferguson DM, Horwood LJ (2000). Does cannabis use encourage other
forms of illicit drug use? Addiction 95:505-520.
Foltin RW, Fischman MW, Brady JV, Bernstein DJ, Capriotti RM, Nellis
MJ, Kelly TH (1990). Motivational effects of smoked marijuana:
behavioral contingencies and low-probability activities. J Exp Anal
Behav 53:5-19.
Ford RD, Balster RL, Dewey WL, Beckner JS (1977). Delta-9-THC and
11-OH-delta-9-THC: behavioral effects and relationship to plasma and
brain levels. Life Sci 20:1993-2003.
Gardner EL (1992). Cannabinoid interactions with brain reward
systems–The Neurobiological basis of cannabinoid abuse. In: Murphy
L, & Bartke A (Eds), Marijuana/Cannabinoids: Neurobiology and
Neurophysiology. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL; pp. 275-335.
Gardner EL, & Lowinson JH (1991). Marijuana’s interaction with brain
reward systems: Update 1991. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 40:571-580.
Gardner EL, Paredes W, Smith D, Donner A, Milling C, Cohen D, &
Morrison D (1988). Facilitation of brain stimulation reward by
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Psychopharmacology 96:142-144.
Gardner EL, Vorel SR (1998). Cannabinoid transmission and reward-
related events. Neurobiol Dis 5:502-533.
Gauvin DV & Baird TJ (1999). The discriminative stimulus properties
of compound drug stimuli: a focus on attention. Pharmacology,
Biochemistry and Behavior.
Gauvin DV, Cheng EY & Holloway FA (1993). Biobehavioral correlates
of alcohol hangover. In: Galanter, M. (Ed.) Recent Developments in
Alcoholism: Ten Years of Progress NY: Plenum Press, pp. 281-304.
Gauvin DV, Harland RD, & Holloway FA (1989). Drug discrimination
procedures: A method to analyze adaptation level of affective
states. Drug Develop Res 16:183-194.
Gledhill-Hoyt J, Lee H, Strote J, Wechsler H (2000). Increased use
of marijuana and other illicit drugs at US colleges in the 1990s:
results of three national surveys. Addiction 95:1655-1667.
Golub A, Johnson BD (1994). The shifting importance of alcohol and
marijuana as gateway substances among serious drug abusers. Journal
on the Studies of Alcohol 55:607-614.
Goudie AJ (1987). Aversive stimulus properties of drugs: The
conditioned taste aversion paradigm. In: Greenshaw AJ & Dourish CT
(Eds) Experimental Psychopharmacology. Humana Press: Clifton, NJ,
- 341-391.
Guimares FS, Chiarett TM, Graeff FG, & Zuardi AW (1990). Antianxiety
effect of cannabidiol in the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology
100:558-559.
Guimares FS, DeAguiar JC, Mechoulam R, Breuer A (1994). Anxiolytic
effect of cannabidiol derivatives in the elevated plus-maze. Gen
Pharmac 25:161-164.
Gust SW, & Walsh JM (1989). Drugs in the Workplace: Research and
Evaluation Data. NIDA Monograph No. 91. US Government Printing
Office: Washington, DC.
Hamilton HC (1912). The pharmacopoeia requirements for Cannabis
sativa. J Am Pharm Assoc 1:200-203.
Hamilton HC (1915). Cannabis sativa: Is the medicinal value found
only in the Indian grown drug. J Am Pharm Assoc 4:448-451.
Hanus L, Subova D (1989). The amount of main cannabinoid substances
in hemp, cultivated for industrial fibre production and their
changes in the course of one vegetation period. Acta Univ Palacki
Olomuc, Fac Med 122:11-23.
Hanus L, Yoshida T, Kreji (1975). Production of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol from hemp cultivated in climatic conditions of
Czechoslovakia. Acta Univ Palacki Olomuc, Fac Med 74:173-180.
Haney M, Ward AS, Comer SD, Foltin RW, Fischman MW (1999a).
Abstinence symptoms following oral THC administration to humans.
Psychopharmacology 141:385-394.
Haney M, Ward AS, Comer SD, Foltin RW, Fischman MW (1999b).
Abstinence symptoms following smoked marijuana in humans.
Psychopharmacology 141:395-404.
Harris RT, Waters W, & McLendon D (1974). Evaluation of the
reinforcing capability of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in rhesus
monkeys. Psychopharmacologia 37:23.
Harris D, Jones RT, Shank R, Nath R, Fernandez E, Goldstein K,
Mendelson J (2000). Self-reported marijuana effects and
characteristics of 100 San Francisco medical marijuana club members.
J Addict Dis 19:89-103.
Heishman SJ, Huestis MA, Henningfield JE, Cone EJ (1990). Acute and
residual effects of marijuana: profiles of plasma THC levels,
physiological, subjective, and performance measures. Pharmacology
Biochemistry & Behavior 37:561-565.
Henningfield JE (1984). Behavioral pharmacology of cigarette
smoking. In: Thompson T, Dews PB & Barrett JE (Eds), Advances in
Behavioral Pharmacology, Volume 4, Academic Press: Orlando, FL, pp
131-210.
Hiltunen AJ, & Jarbe TUC (1986). Interactions between 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol as evaluated by drug
discrimination procedures in pigeons. Neuropharmacol 25:133-142.
Hiltunen AJ, Jarbe TUC, & Wangdahl K (1988). Cannabinol and
cannabidiol in combination: temperature, open-field activity, and
vocalization. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 30:675-682.
Hines B, Torrelio M, & Gershon S (1975a). Interactions between
cannabinol and cannabidiol during abstinence in morphine-dependent
rats. Res Comm Chem Pathol Pharmacol 12:185-188.
Hine B, Torrelio M, & Gershon S. (1975b). Differential effects of
cannabidiol and 9-THC during abstinence in morphine-
dependent rats. Life Sci 17:185-188.
Ho BT, Estevez VS, Englert LF (1973). The uptake and metabolic fate
of cannabinoids in rat brains. J Pharm Pharmacol 25:488-490.
Hoffman AF, Lupica CR (2001). Direct actions of cannabinoids on
synaptic transmission in the Nucleus Accumbens: A comparison with
opioids. J Physiol 85:72-83.
Hollister LE (1974). Structure activity relationship in man of
cannabis constituents and homologs and metabolites of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol. Pharmacology 11:3-11.
Hollister LE (1988). Cannabis–1988.(Literature Review). Acta
Psychiatr Scand 78:108-118.
Hollister LE (1986). Health aspects of cannabis. Pharmacol Rev 38:1-
20.
Hollister LE, & Gellespie BA (1975). Interactions in man of delta-9-
tetrahydrocannabinol. II. Cannabinol and cannabidiol. Clin Pharmacol
Therap 18:80-83.
Howlett AC (1987). Cannabinoid inhibition of adenylate cyclase:
relative activities of marihuana constituents and metabolites.
Neuropharmacology 26:507-512.
Howlett AC, Evans DM, & Houston DB (1992). The cannabinoid receptor.
In: Murphy L & Bartke A (Eds) Marijuana/Cannabinoids: Neurobiology
and Neurophysiology. Boca Raton: CRC Press, pp 38-72.
Huffman JW, Dai D, Martin BR, & Compton DR (1994). Design, synthesis
and pharmacology of cannabimimetic indoles. BioMed Chem Lett 4:563-
566.
Institute of Medicine (1982). Division of Health Sciences Policy.
Marijuana and Health: Report of a study by committee of the
Institute of Medicine, Washington, D.C. National Academy Press.
Institute of Medicine (1999). Marijuana and medicine: Assessing the
science base. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Isbell H, Gorodetzky CW, Jasinski D, Claussen U, VonSpulak F, &
Korte F (1967). Effects of (-)-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in
man. Psychopharmacologia 11:184-188.
Izquierdo I, Tannhauser M (1973). Letter: The effect of cannabidiol
on maximal electroshock seizures in rats. J Pharm Pharmacol 25:916-
917.
Jarbe TU, Hiltunen AJ, & Mechoulam R (1989). Subjectively
experienced cannabis effects in animals. Drug Develop Res 16:385-
393.
Jarbe TU, & Hendricksson BG (1974). Discriminative response control
produced by hashish, tetrahydrocannabinols ( 8-THC and
9-THC) and other drugs. Psychopharmacologia (Berl.) 40:1-
16.
Jarbe TU, Hendricksson BG, & Ohlin GC (1977). 9-THC as a
discriminative cue in pigeon: effects of 8-THC, CBD, and
CBN. Arch Internat Pharmacodyn Ther 228:68-72.
[[Page 20074]]
Jarbe TU, & Mathis DA (1992). Dissociative and discriminative
stimulus functions of cannabinoids/cannabimimetics. In: Murphy L &
Bartke A (Eds), Marijuana/Cannabinoids: Neurobiology and
Neurophysiology. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, pp. 425-458.
Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, & Bachman JG (1996). National Survey
Results on Drug Abuse from the Monitoring the Future Study, 1975-
- Volume 1: Secondary School Students. U.S. Government Printing
Office: Washington, DC.
Jones RT (1971). Marijuana-induced “high”: influence of
expectation, setting and previous drug experience. Pharmacol Rev
23:359-369.
Jones RT (1980). Human effects: an overview. In: Petersen RC (Ed)
Marijuana research findings: 1980. NIDA Res Mono 31. U.S. Govt
Printing Office: Washington DC, pp 54-79.
Jones RT, Benowitz NL, & Herning RI (1981). Clinical relevance of
cannabis tolerance and dependence. J Clin Pharmacol 21:143S-152S.
Jones RT, Pertwee RG (1972). A metabolic interaction in vivo between
cannabidio and delta-1-tetrahydrocannabinol. Br J Pharmacol 45:375-
377.
Kamien JB, Bickel WK, Higgins ST, & Hughes JR (1994). The effects of
9-tetrahydrocannabinol on repeated acquisition and
performance of response sequences and on self-reports in humans.
Behav Pharmacol 5:71-78.
Karniol IG, & Carlini EA (1972). The content of (-) 9-
trans-tetrahydrocannabinol (9-THC) does not explain all
biological activity of some Brazilian marijuana samples. J Pharm
Pharmacol 24:833-835.
Karniol IG, & Carlini EA (1973). Comparative studies in man and in
laboratory animals on 8-and 9-trans-tetrahydrocannabinol.
Pharmacology 9:115-126.
Karniol IG, & Carlini EA (1973). Pharmacological interaction between
cannabidiol and 9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Psychopharmacologia
(Berl) 33:53-70.
Karniol IG, Shirakawa I, Kasinski N, Pfefferman A, & Carlini EA
(1974). Cannabidiol interferes with the effects of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol in man. Eur J Pharmacol 28:172-178.
Kaymakcalan S (1973).Tolerance and dependence on cannabis. Bull Narc
25:39-47.
Kelly P, & Jones RT (1992). Metabolism of tetrahydrocannabinol in
frequent and infrequent marijuana users. J Anal Toxicol 16:328-335.
Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Emurian CS, Fischman MW (1997). Are choice and
self-administration of marijuana related to delta-9-THC content? Exp
Clin Psychopharmacol 5:74-82.
Kelly TH, Foltin RW, & Fischman MW (1993). Effects of smoked
marijuana on heart rate, drug ratings and task performance by
humans. Behav Pharmacol 4:167-178.
Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Mayr MT, & Fischman MW (1994). Effects of
9-tetrahydrocannabinol and social context on marijuana
self-administration by humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 49:763-768.
Kessler R, McGonagle K, Zhao S, Nelson, CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S,
Wittchen H-U, Kendler KS (1994). Lifetime and 12 month prevalence of
DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from
the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:8-19.
Kiplinger GF, Manno JE, Rodda BE, Fornery RB, Haine SE, East R,
Richards AB (1971). Dose-response analysis of the effects of
tetrahydrocannabinol in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther 12:650-657.
Koob GF (1992). Neural mechanisms of drug reinforcement. Ann NY Acad
Sci 654:171-191.
Koob GF, Roberts AJ, Schulteis G, Parsons LH, Heyser CJ, Hyytia P,
Merlo-Pich E, Weiss F (1998). Neurocircuitry targets in ethanol
reward and dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:3-9.
Khouri EM, Pope HG, Lukas SE (1999). Changes in aggressive behavior
during withdrawal from long-term marijuana use. Psychopharmacology
143:302-308.
Kouri EM, Pope HG (2000) Abstinence symptoms during withdrawal from
chronic marijuana use. Exper Clin Psychopharmacol 8: 483-492.
Kurzthaler I, Hummer M, Miller C, Sperner-Unterweger B, Gunther V,
Wechdorn H, Battista H-J, Fleischhacker WW (1999). Effect of
cannabis use on cognitive functions and driving ability. J Clin
Psychiatry 60:395-399.
Lemberger L, Crabtree R, Rowe HM (1972). 11-hydroxy-9-
tetrahydrocannabinol: pharmcology, disposition, and metabolism of a
major metabolite of marihuana in man. Science 177:62-64.
Lemberger L, & Rubin A. (1975). The physiologic disposition of
marijuana in man. Life Sci 17:1637-1642.
Lepore M, Vorel SR, Lowinson J, Gardner EL (1995). Conditioned place
preference induced by 9-tetrahydrocannabinol: Comparison
with cocaine, morphine and food reward. Life Sci 56:2073-2080.
Lichtman A, & Martin BR (1996). 9-tetrahydrocannabinol
impairs spatial memory through a cannabinoid receptor mechanism.
Psychopharmacology 126:125-131.
Little PJ, Compton DR, Johnson MR, Melvin LS, & Martin BR (1988).
Pharmacology and stereoselectivity of structurally novel
cannabinoids in mice. J Pharmacol Exper Ther 247:1046.
Lukas SE, Mendelson JH, Benedikt R (1995). Electroencephalographic
correlates of marihuana-induced euphoria. Drug Alcohol Depend
37:131-140.
Machula IA, Dudkin SM, & Barkov NK (1992). Characterization of
mechanisms mediating the effects of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol
on behavior. In: Murphy L & Bartke A (Eds), Marijuana/Cannabinoids.
Neurobiology and Neurophysiology. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL; pp.
525-538.
Manno JE, Kiplinger GF, Scholz N, Forney RB, Haine SE (1971). The
influence of alcohol and marihuana on motor and mental performance.
Clin Pharmacol Ther 12:202-211.
Manno JE, Kiplinger GF, Haine SE, Bennett IF, Forney RB (1970).
Comparative effects of smoking marihuana on placebo on human motor
and mental performance. Clin Pharmacol Ther 11:808-815.
Martin BR, Balster RL, Razdan RK, Harris LS, & Dewey WL (1981).
Behavioral comparisons of stereoisomers of tetrahydrocannabinols.
Life Sci 29:565.
Martin BR, Compton DR, Prescott WR, Barrett RL, & Razdan RK (1995).
Pharmacological evaluation of dimethylheptyl analogs of 9-
THC: reassessment of the putative three-point cannabinoid-receptor
interaction. Drug Alcohol Depend 37:231-240.
Martin BR, Hall W (1997, 1998). The health effects of cannabis: key
issues of policy relevance. Bulletin on Narcotics XLIX & L (1&2):85-
116.
Martin G, Nie Z, Siggens GR (1997). Mu-Opioid receptors modulate
NMDA receptor-mediated responses in nucleus accumbens neurons J
Neurosci 17:11-22.
Mechoulam R (1973). Marijuana: Chemistry, pharmacology, metabolism,
and clinical effects. NY: Academic Press.
Mechoulam R (1998). Endocannabinoids. Eur J Pharmacol 359:1-18.
Mechoulum R, Shani A, Edery HM, & Grunfield Y (1970). Chemical basis
for hashish activity. Science 169:611-612.
Mello NK (1989). Drug self-administration procedures: Alcohol and
marijuana. In: Fischman MW, & Mello NR (Eds). Testing for Abuse
Liability of Drugs in Humans. US Government Printing Office:,
Washington, DC; pp.147-170.
Mello NK, & Mendelson JH (1985). Operant acquisition of marijuana by
women. J Pharmacol Exper Therap 235:162-171.
Mendelson JH, & Mello NK (1984). Reinforcing properties of oral
9-tetrahydrocannabinol, smoked marijuana and
nabilone: Influence of previous marijuana use. Psychopharmacology
83:351-356.
Mendelson JH, & Mello NK (1984). Effects of marijuana on
neuroendocrine hormones in human males and females. In Braude, M.C.
and Ludford, J.P., (Eds). Marijuana Effects on the Endocrine and
Reproductive Systems. National Institute on Drug Abuse Monograph 44.
DHHS Pub No. (ADM) 84-1278. U.S. Printing Office: Washington, DC.
Mendelson JH, Rossi AM, & Meyer RE (1974). The Use of Marijuana: A
Psychological and Physiological Inquiry. Plenum Press: New York.
Microgram. 30: 1, 1997.
Miller LL, Cocchetto DM, & Perez-Reyes M (1983). Relationship
between several pharmacokinetic parameters and psychometric indices
of subjective effects of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in man. Eur
J Pharmacol 25:633-637.
Monti JM (1977). Hypnotic-like effects of cannabidiol in the rat.
Psychopharmacology 55:263-265.
Musty RE (1984). Possible anxiolytic effects of cannabidiol. In:
Agurell S, Dewey WL, Willette RE (Eds) The cannabinoids: chemical,
pharmacologic, and
[[Page 20075]]
therapeutic aspects. NY: Academic Press, pp. 795-815.
Musty RE, & Sands R (1978). Effects of marijuana extract distillate
and cannabidiol on variable interval performance as a function of
food deprivation. Pharmacology 16:199-205.
Musty RE, Reggio P, & Consroe P (1995). A review of recent advances
in cannabinoid research and the 1994 International Symposium on
Cannabis and the Cannabinoids. Life Sci 56:1933-1940.
Nakamura EM, da Silva EA, Concilio GM, Wilkinson DA, & Masur J
(1991). Reversible effects of acute and long-term administration of
tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) on memory in the rat. Drug
Alcohol Depend 28:167-175.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2000a). Marijuana
and alcohol combined severely impede driving performance. Ann Emerg
Med 35:398-399.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2000b). NHTSA
Technical Report #225.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (1999). NHTSA
Technical Report #201.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (1998). NHTSA
Technical Report #185.
National Institute in Drug Abuse (1996). Conference Highlights.
National Conference on Marijuana Use: Prevention, Treatment, and
Research. July 19-20, 1995. Sponsored by National Institute in Drug
Abuse, National Institutes of Health, NIH Publication No, 96:96-
4106.
NCADI: 1996 DAWN Survey.
Nelson K, Walsh D, Deeter P, & Sheehan F (1994). A Phase II study of
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol for appetite stimulation in cancer-
associated anorexia. J Palliat Care 10:14-18.
Nemeth-Coslett R, Henningfield JE, O’Keefe MK, & Griffiths RR
(1986). Effects of marijuana smoking on suvjective ratings of
tobacco smothing. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 25:569-665.
Nilsson I, Agurell S, Nilsson JLG, Widman M, Leander K (1973). Two
cannabidiol metabolites formed by rat liver. J Pharm Pharmacol 25:
486-487.
Onaivi ES, Green MR, Martin BR (1990). Pharmacological
characterization of cannabinoids in the elevated plus maze. J
Pharmacol Exp Ther 253: 1002-1009.
Paris M, Nahas GG (1984). Botany: the unstabilized species. In:
Nahas GG (Ed.) Marihuana in science and medicine. NY: Raven Press,
pp 3-36.
Paris M, Boucher F, & Cosson L (1975). Importance des compose a
chaine propylique dans le Cannabis originaire d’Afrique du Sud.
Plantes Med Phytother 9:136-139.
Parker LA, & Gillies T (1995). THC-induced place and taste aversions
in Lewis and Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav Neurosci 109:71-78.
Patton WDM, & Pertwee RG. (1973). The actions of cannabis in man.
In: Mechoulam R (Ed), Marijuana: chemistry, pharmacology,
metabolism, and clinical effects. Academic Press: New York, pp 287-
333.
Perez-Reyes M, Simmons J, Brine D, Kimmel GL, Davis KH, Wall ME
(1976). Rate of penetration of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and 11-
hydroxy-delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol to the brain of mice. In: Nahas
G, Paton WDM, Idanpaan-Heikkila JE (Eds), Marihuana: chemistry,
biochemistry, and cellular effects. Springer-Verlag: New York, pp
179-185.
Perez-Reyes M, Timmons MC, Davis KH, & Wall EM (1973). A comparison
of the pharmacological activity in man of intravenously administered
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabinol, and cannabidiol.
Experientia 29:1368-1369.
Perez-Reyes M, White WR, McDonald SA, Hicks RE, Jeffcoat AR, Cook CE
(1991). The pharmacologic effects of daily marijuana smoking in
humans. Phamacol Biochem Behav 40: 691-694.
Perio A, Rinaldi-Carmona M, Maruani J, Barth F, LeFur G, & Soubrie P
(1996). Central mediation of the cannabinoid cue: activity of a
selective CB1 antagonist, SR 141716A. Behav Pharmacol
7:65-71.
Pertwee RG (1991) Tolerance to and dependence on psychotropic
cannabinoids. In: The Biological Bases of Drug Tolerance and
Dependence. Academic press: New York; pp. 231-263.
Phillips RN, Turk RF, & Forney RB (1971). Acute toxicity of delta-9-
tetrahydrocannabinol in rats and mice. Proc Soc Exper Biol Med
136:260.
Physicians Desk Reference, 51st edition. (1997). Medical Economics
Company, Inc., Monvale, New Jersey, pp. 2353-2355.
Pickens R, Thompson T, & Muchow DC (1973). Cannabis and
phencyclidine self-administered by animals. In:Goldfarb L, &
Hoffmeister F (Eds) Psychic Dependence (Bayer-Symposium IV).
Springer-Verlag, Berlin; pp. 78.
Pope HG, & Yurgelun-Todd D (1996). The residual cognitive effects of
heavy marijuana use in college students. JAMA 275:521-527.
Pradhan SN (1984). Pharmacology of some synthetic
tetrahydrocannabinols. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 8:369-385.
Preston KL, Walsh SL, & Sannerud CA (1997). Indirect measures
related to drug reinforcement. In: Johnson BA, & Roache J (Eds),
Drug Addiction and its Treatment: Nexus of Neuroscience and
Behavior. Raven Press:New York; pp 91-114.
Razdan RK (1986). Structrue-activity relationships in cannabinoids.
Pharmacol Rev 38:75.
Razdan RK, & Howes JF (1983). Drugs related to tetrahydrocannabinol.
Med Res Rev 3:119-146.
Report to the Director, National Institutes of Health, by the Ad Hoc
Group of Experts, (1997). Workshop on the Medical Utility of
Marijuana, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD February 19-
20, 1997, available on the NIH Homepage http://www.nih.gov/news/
medmarijuana/medicalmarijuana.html Date: July 25, 1997.
Sanders J, Jackson DM, & Starmer GA (1979). Interactions among the
cannabinoids in the antagonism of the abdominal constriction
response in the mouse. Psychopharmacology 61:281-285.
Sarafian TA, Marques-Magallanes JA, Shau H, Tashkin D, Roth MD
(1999). Oxidative stress produced by marijuana smoke: an adverse
effect enhanced by cannabinoids. Am J Respir Cell Mol Biol 20: 1286-
1293.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (1999).
Federal study links wide range of behavior problems to marijuana use
by teens. A SAMHSA report: adolescent self-reported behaviors and
their association with marijuana use. http://www.samhsa.gov/press/
980922fs.htm.
Takahashi RN, & Singer G (1979). Self-administration of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol by rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 11:737.
Tanda G, Munzar P, Goldberg SR (2000). Self-administration behavior
maintained by the psychoactive ingredient of marijuana in squirrel
monkeys. Nature Neurosci 3: 1073-1074.
Tart CT (1971). On Being Stoned: A Psychological Study of Marijuana
Intoxication. Science and Behavior Books: Palo Alto, CA.
Taylor DR, Poulton R, Moffitt TE, Ramankutty P, Sears MR (2000). The
respiratory effects of cannabis dependence in young adults.
Addiction 95:1669-1677.
Ten Ham M, & DeJong Y (1975). Absence of interaction between
9-tetrahydrocannabinol 9-THC) and cannabidiol in
aggression, muscle control, and body temperature experiments in
mice. Psychopharmacologia (Berl) 41:169-174.
Thomas BF, Adams IB, Mascarella SW, Martin BR, & Razdan RK (1996).
Structure -activity analysis of anandamide analogs: Relationship to
a cannabinoid pharmacophore. J Med Chem 39:471-479.
Thompson GW, et al. (1970-1972). Determine toxicity of delta-8 and
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and marijuana extract. Mason Research
Institute, Worcester, Massachusetts Reports I-XIX to the National
Institutes of Mental Health. Contract No. HSM 42-70-95 (June 1970-
June 1971) and No. HSM 42-71-79 (June 1971-January 1972).
Tsou K, Patrick SL, & Walker JM (1995). Physical withdrawal in rats
tolerant to 9-tetrahydrocannabinol precipitated by a
cannabinoid receptor antagonist. Eur J Pharmacol 280:R13-R15.
Turner CE (1980). Chemistry and metabolism. In: Petersen RC (Ed)
Marijuana research findings: 1980. NIDA Res Mono 31. U.S. Gov’t
Printing Office: Washington DC, pp 81-97.
Turner CE (1980). Marijuana research and problems: an overview.
Pharmac Internat May: 93-96.
Turner CE, ElSohly MA, & Boeren EG (1980a). Constituents of cannabis
sativa
[[Page 20076]]
- XVII. A review of the natural constituents. J Nat Prod 43:169-
234.
Turner CE, Elsohly MA, Boeren EG (1980b). Constituents of Cannabis
sativa. XV. Botanical and chemical profile of Indian variant. Planta
Med 37:217-225.
Turner CE, Elsohly MA, Lewis GS, Lopez-Santibanez I, Carranza J
(1982). Constituents of Cannabis sative L., XX: the cannabinoid
content of Mexican variants grown in Mexico and in Mississippi,
United States of America. Bull Narc 34:45-59.
Turner JC, Hemphill JK, Mahlberg PG (1980). Trichomes and
cannabinoid content of developing leaves and bracts of Cannabis
sativa L., Cannabacceae. Am J Bot 67:1397-1406.
U.S. Department of Justice. Drug Enforcement Administration (1994).
Cannabis Investigations Section. 1993 Domestic cannabis Eradication/
Suppression Program. Washington, DC.
U.N. Division of Narcotic Drugs (1974). The chemistry of cannabis
and its components. MNAR/9/1974-GE, 74-11502.
U.N. International Narcotics Control Board (1994). Psychotropic
Substances, Statistics for 1993. United Nations Publication, Vienna,
Austria, pp. 39-42.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1995). National
Household Survey on Drug Abuse. Main Findings, 1993, U.S. Government
Printing Office, Washington, DC 1995.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1995). National
Household Survey on Drug Abuse. Population Estimates 1994, U.S.
Government Printing Office, Washington, DC.
Vachon L, Sulkowski A, & Rich E. (1974). Marihuana effects on
learning, attention and time estimation. Psychopharmacology 39:1-11.
Wall ME, Perez-Reyes M (1981). The metabolism of delta-9-
tetrahydrocannabinol and related cannabinoids in man. J Clin
Pharmacol 21:178S-189S.
Welburn PJ, Starmer GA, Chesher GB, & Jackson DM (1976). Effets of
cannbinoids on the abdominal constriction response in mice: within
cannbinoid interactions. Psychopharmacologia (Berl.) 46:83-85.
Weil AT, & Zinberg NE (1969). Acute effects of marihuana on speech.
Nature 222:434-437.
Weil AT, Zinberg NE, & Nelsen JM (1968). Clinical and psychological
effects of marihuana in man. Science 162:1231-1242.
Wiley JL, Barrett RL, Balster RL, & Martin BR (1993a). Tolerance to
the discriminative stimulus effects of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol. Behav Pharmacol 4:581-585.
Wiley JL, Barrett RL, Britt DT, Balster RL, & Martin BR (1993b).
Discriminative stimulus effects of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol and 9-11-
tetrahydrocannabinol in rats and rhesus monkeys. Neuropharmacology
32:359-365.
Wiley JL, Huffman JW, Balster RL, & Martin BR (1995a).
Pharmacological specificity of the discriminative stimulus effects
of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in rhesus monkeys. Drug Alcohol
Depend 40:81-86.
Wiley JL, Lowe JA, Balster RL, & Martin BR (1995b). Antagonism of
the discriminative stimulus effects of 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol in rats and rhesus monkeys. J Pharmacol Exper
Therap 275:1-6.
Wise RA (1996). Neurobiology of addiction. Curr Opin Neurobiol
6:243-251.
Wise RA, Bozarth MA (1987). A psychomotor stimulant theory of
addiction. Psychol Rev 94:469-492.
Wu X, French ED (2000). Effects of chronic 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol on rat midbrain dopamine neurons: an
electrophysiological assessment. Neruopharmacology 39:391-398.
Yesavage A, Leirer VO, Denari M, Hollister LE (1985). Carry-over
effects of marijuana intoxication on aircraft pilot performance: A
preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry 142:1325-1329.
Yuan XR, Madamba S, Siggens GR (1992). Opioid peptides reduce
synaptic transmission in the nucleus accumbens. Neurosci Lett
134:223-228.
Zacny JP, Chait LD (1989). Breathhold duration and response to
marijuana smoke. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 33:481-484.
Zacny JP, Chait LD (1991). Response to marijuana as a function of
potency and breathhold duration. Psychopharmacology 103:223-226.
Zhang Z-F, Morgenstern H, Spitz MR, Tashkin DP, Yu G-P, Marshall R,
Hsu TC, Schantz SP (1999). Marijuana use and increased risk of
squamous cell carcinoma of the head and neck. Cancer Epidemiol
Biomarkers & Prevent 8:1071-1078.
Zuardi AW, Antunes Rodriguez J, & Cunha JM (1991). Effects of
cannabidiol in animal models predictive of antipsychotic activity.
Psychopharmacology 104:260-264.
Zuardi AW, & Karniol IG (1983). Effect on variable-interval
performance in rats of “9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol,
separately and in combination. Brazil J Med Biol Res 16:141-146.
Zuardi AW, Finkelfarb E, Bueno OFA, Musty RE, & Karniol IG (1981).
Characteristics of the stimulus produced by the mixture of
cannabidiol with 9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Arch Internat
Pharmacodynam Ther 249:137-146.
Zuardi AW, Morais SL, Guimaraes FS, Mechoulam R (1995).
Antipsychotic effect of cannabidiol. J Clin Psychiatry 56:485-486.
Zuardi AW, Shirakawa I, Finkelfarb E, & Karniol IG (1982). Action of
cannabidiol on the anxiety and other effects produced by 9-
THC in normal subjects. Psychopharmacology 76:245-250.
Zuardi AW, Teixeira NA, & Karniol IG (1984). Pharmacological inter
action of the effects of 9-trans-tetrahydrocannabinol and
cannabidiol on serum corticisterone levels in the rat. Arch Internat
Pharmacodyn Ther 269:12-19.